Shooting bias explained

The term shooting bias, also known as "shooter bias", is a proposed form of implicit racial bias which refers to the apparent tendency among the police to shoot black civilians more often than white civilians, even when they are unarmed.[1] In countries where white people aren't the majority, shooting bias may still apply, with different minority groups facing discrimination.

The concept proposes that the probability of being shot by the police depends on ethnicity in addition to the other known factors like location, the income of the neighborhood and whether or not the person is carrying a weapon[2] as well as the emotions shown by the victim.[3] Shooting bias is not limited to one race, as studies have shown that both black and white individuals demonstrated almost equivalent levels of shooting bias.[4]

Key Questions in Literature

Benchmark

Violent crime is committed by a very small number of individuals. These individuals tend to come from very poor disadvantaged communities. Since minorities are overrepresented in these communities, minorities are also overrepresented in violent crime. Thus, any statistical analysis of police bias must take this into account. Using population, police-citizen interactions, or total arrests as a benchmark, we observe that black citizens appear more likely than white citizens to be fatally shot by police officers. Using violent crime arrests or weapons offense arrests, we observe that black citizens appear less likely to be fatally shot by police officers.[5]

Resisting Arrest

Additionally, data on the percent of individuals who resist arrest is also needed to draw conclusions about police bias. That data was available for Texas and California. Researchers were able to analyze this data to determine that there was shooting bias in California but not in Texas.[6]

Number of deaths

Problems with police reporting practices

Departments can voluntarily include justifiable homicides in the crime statistics of the FBI's Uniform Crime Reports, which means that a lot of departments don't provide data at all. Some cities haven't reported their data in years.[7] This means that the official data doesn't accurately reflect the number of civilians that are shot by the police.[8]

Newspapers like The Guardian and The Washington Post have started compiling databases of fatal police shootings,[9] [10] revealing that in 2015 twice as many civilians had been fatally shot as the FBI's data suggested.[11] An FBI working group has started working on a proposal for making the reports more accurate, but they would still rely on voluntary data and therefore wouldn't fix the main reporting issue.[12]

Alternative database by The Guardian

The database developed by The Guardian is currently the largest database on fatal shootings available.[13] They gather data through police reports, monitoring of regional news, fact-checked witness statements and other crowdsourced police fatality databases.[14]

Number of deaths

In 2016, The Guardian counted 1093 people who were killed by the police in the United States. Out of these, 574 were white and 266 were black. 95 of the white victims were unarmed, while 42 of the black victims were unarmed.

More white than black people are shot. It is important to distinguish to differentiate between the number of deaths of an ethnic group and the likelihood of being shot by police. The likelihood of being shot as a black rather than a white person is higher, whether the victim is armed or not.

Likelihood of being shot with respect to race

Observational studies

A study carried out at the University of California found "evidence of a significant bias in the killing of unarmed black Americans compared to unarmed white Americans".[2] In this study, the probability of being shot by the police as a black, unarmed person versus as a white, unarmed person was 3.49 times higher. Unarmed Hispanics' likelihood to be shot was 1.67 times higher than for unarmed Whites. Black people have been 28% of those killed by police since 2013 despite being only 13% of the population.[15]

On the other hand a 2016–2018 study by the National Bureau of Economic Research of twenty-first century data recorded by 12 police departments across the U.S., which took into account various factors such as type of confrontation, whether the suspect was armed or not, and whether or not they drew a gun, found that while overall "blacks are 21 percent more likely than whites to be involved in an interaction with police in which at least a weapon is drawn" and that in the raw data from New York City's Stop and Frisk program "blacks and Hispanics are more than fifty percent more likely to have an interaction with police which involves any use of force" after "[p]artitioning the data in myriad ways, we find no evidence of racial discrimination in officer-involved shootings." The study did find bias against blacks and Hispanics in non-lethal and less-extreme lethal violence, stating that "as the intensity of force increases (e.g. handcuffing civilians without arrest, drawing or pointing a weapon, or using pepper spray or a baton), the probability that any civilian is subjected to such treatment is small, but the racial difference remains surprisingly constant", and noted that "[u]ntil recently, data on officer-involved shootings were extremely rare and contained little information on the details surrounding an incident".

After the NBER study was published in the peer-reviewed Journal of Political Economy, a comment on it by Steven Durlauf and (Nobel Memorial Prize in Economics recipient) James Heckman of the Harris School of Public Policy Studies at the University of Chicago stated, "[i]n our judgment, this paper does not establish credible evidence on the presence or absence of discrimination against African Americans in police shootings."[16] The NBER study's author, Roland G. Fryer Jr., responded by saying Durlauf and Heckman erroneously claim that his sample is "based on stops". Further, he states that the "vast majority of the data...is gleaned from 911 calls for service in which a civilian requests police presence."[17] A 2018 study in the journal Social Psychological and Personality Science sought to "argue for more reasonable benchmarks to compare fatal shooting rates across racial groups" than "comparing the group's raw shooting numbers against each group's overall representation in the population". On the premise that "the more [racial] group members are involved in criminal activity, the more exposure they have to situations in which police shootings would be likely to occur" the authors analyzed The Guardian database through their own measurements of each race's involvement in criminal activity, calculated from the Federal Bureau of Investigation's Summary Reporting System (SRS) and National Incident-Based Reporting System (NIBRS), the National Crime Victimization Survey (NCVS), and the Centers for Disease Control and Prevention's WONDER database. Once the data were weighted by these measurements, the authors were able to conclude that, for example, although "[o]dds were 3.7 times higher for Blacks relative to Whites to be fatally shot given population proportions" while holding or reaching for a harmless object, this was appropriate given higher rates of average criminal involvement among blacks. They propose, "[i]f officers are more likely to misidentify a harmless object in the hands of a Black citizen due to stereotypes, the cause of officers holding those stereotypes may rest with (the very small percentage of) those who are more likely to engage in criminal activity."[18]

A more recent study was conducted by Michigan State University and the University of Maryland, compiling a list of more than 900 fatal U.S. police shootings in 2015 using crowdsourced databases from The Washington Post and The Guardian.https://www.pnas.org/content/116/32/15877 Then, they asked police departments for information about the race of the officers responsible for the shootings. They found black police were more likely to kill black civilians than white civilians. However, the same held true for white and Hispanic officers: Each group of police was more likely to shoot civilians of their own race. Researchers claimed this is true because police tend to be drawn from the communities they work in and are thus more likely to have deadly encounters with civilians of the same race. They conclude that "increasing diversity among officers by itself is unlikely to reduce racial disparity in police shootings".

Experimental studies

Several experimental studies by social psychologists, in which college students are tested playing computer game simulations, have uncovered racial bias in their decisions to shoot.[19] [20] [21] [22] A computer game simulation experiment in 2010 that tested college students, community members, and police officers found that racial bias was moderated by "prototypicality", i.e. they were more likely to shoot if a person had stronger features generally associated with black people (broader nose, large lips, etc.) regardless of the suspects actual racial category/skin colour, to the extent that white suspects with strong "black features" suffered more negative bias than black suspects with strong "white features". Furthermore they found greater racial bias among college students and community members than police officers (who are trained to overcome racial bias), and on average could not find racial bias among the police officers tested (although there was prototypicality bias).[23]

Factors that influence decision to shoot

List of empirically researched factors

Empirical research suggests the following factors to influence the decision to shoot:

Implicit racial bias

An implicit racial bias refers to unintentional judgments a person makes of a group (e.g. good/bad) of a certain ethnicity. Therefore, a person who shows implicit racial bias might not be aware of it. Police officers have been found to show a racial bias against black people in the decision to shoot,[28] and implicit biases are directly correlated with shooting behavior.

In one of the studies, researchers investigated how stereotypes affected police officers' decisions to shoot. They used a video game and exposed their participants to pictures of either Whites or Non-Whites who were armed or unarmed. During this video game, the participants were asked to choose between "shoot" or "don't shoot" as quickly as possible. The results were that the participants shot armed black people faster than armed white people and chose "don't shoot" faster for unarmed white than unarmed black persons.

The authors explained those findings by the activation of stereotype thinking which lead white people to associate black people with danger. Because of this stereotype, the participants expected blacks to carry a gun and therefore were quicker to make the "shooting" decision.[29] Other studies have found similar results.[30] [31] The time pressure to make a decision in decisions to shoot might magnify the effects of racial bias.[32] However, more recent research indicates that despite scoring moderately high on an Implicit Association Test, police officers are less likely to shoot black people in a video simulation[33]

Location and context

In the USA, police officers live and work in a society where prejudices against minorities are common. In addition, police officers usually deal with higher crime rates in minority neighborhoods. These experiences tend to reinforce existing prejudices, leading to discrimination against minorities. Other factors that lead to discrimination by police are language barriers between police and some ethnic groups, experiences with disrespectful or hostile residents in certain minority neighborhoods, and low punishment for police officers who misbehave towards minorities.

Due to these prejudices, minorities may develop hostile attitudes towards police and be more likely to behave disrespectfully or to insult officers. This may lead to a self-reinforcing cycle, where the biases of either group reinforce those of the other.[34]

Examples that showcase prejudices among police officers include:

Ways to reduce shooting bias

Possibly the biggest change could be made by holding police officers accountable for their actions.[35] Internal investigations have been criticized for bias in favor of officers, leading to a lack of punishment. The work culture within some police departments has been criticized as racist, in light of many recent scandals.[36] [37] [38]

Knowing the factors that influence police officers decisions to shoot there are a few possible solutions for reducing shooting bias. The main factor is implicit racial bias, which in turn becomes augmented or intensified by certain factors that could be addressed.

Diversity in police departments might not reduce shooting bias,[39] but reducing fatigue might lower the impact of racial bias on the decision to shoot. Changing the training of police officers so as to not showcase black armed targets more often than white ones could help reduce racial bias.[40] [41]

Training police officers in making decisions under stress as well as assigning officers to certain locations so they can become familiar with its residents could reduce the numbers of civilians killed, says Richmond's police chief, Chris Magnus.[42]

Racial bias can be contagious within a social group or neighborhood.[43] Training police officers to be aware of this might help in reducing this effect.[44]

See also

References

  1. Feingold. Jonathan. Lorang. Karen. 2012-01-01. Defusing Implicit Bias. UCLA Law Review Discourse. 59. 212.
  2. Ross. Cody T.. 2015-11-05. A Multi-Level Bayesian Analysis of Racial Bias in Police Shootings at the County-Level in the United States, 2011–2014. PLOS ONE. 10. 11. e0141854. 2015PLoSO..1041854R. 10.1371/journal.pone.0141854. 4634878. 26540108. free.
  3. Kubota. Jennifer T.. Ito. Tiffany A.. The role of expression and race in weapons identification. Emotion. en. 14. 6. 1115–1124. 10.1037/a0038214. 4235656. 25401289. 2014.
  4. Benforado. Adam. 2010-10-29. Quick on the Draw: Implicit Bias and the Second Amendment . en . 1701089.
  5. Tregle. Brandon. Nix. Justin. Alpert. Geoffrey P.. 2018-12-07. Disparity does not mean bias: making sense of observed racial disparities in fatal officer-involved shootings with multiple benchmarks. Journal of Crime and Justice. 42. 1. 18–31. 10.1080/0735648x.2018.1547269. 150005614. 0735-648X.
  6. Shjarback. John A.. Nix. Justin. January 2020. Considering violence against police by citizen race/ethnicity to contextualize representation in officer-involved shootings. Journal of Criminal Justice. 66. 101653. 10.1016/j.jcrimjus.2019.101653. 0047-2352.
  7. News: Nobody counts police killings in the U.S.. Byers. Christine. Moskop . Walker. stltoday.com. 2017-07-30. en. 2014-11-09.
  8. Klinger. David A.. 2011-12-13. On the Problems and Promise of Research on Lethal Police Violence. Homicide Studies. en. 16. 1. 78–96. 10.1177/1088767911430861. 145366158.
  9. Web site: The Counted: people killed by police in the United States – interactive. Swaine. Jon. Laughland. Oliver. the Guardian. 2017-07-30. Lartey. Jamiles. Davis. Kenan. Harris. Rich. Popovich. Nadja. Powell. Kenton. team. Guardian US interactive.
  10. News: Police shootings 2017 database. Washington Post. 2017-07-30.
  11. News: Fatal police shootings in 2015 approaching 400 nationwide . Kindy . Kimberly . 2015-05-30 . Washington Post . 2019-12-08 . Tate . Julie . en-US . Jenkins . Jennifer . Rich . Steven . Alexander . Keith L. . Lowery . Wesley . 1.
  12. News: FBI to sharply expand system for tracking fatal police shootings . Kindy . Kimberly . 2015-12-08 . Washington Post . 2019-12-08 . en-US.
  13. Hehman. Eric. Flake. Jessica K.. Calanchini. Jimmy. 2017-07-27. Disproportionate Use of Lethal Force in Policing Is Associated With Regional Racial Biases of Residents. Social Psychological and Personality Science. 9. 4. en. 393–401. 10.1177/1948550617711229. 51773001.
  14. Web site: About The Counted: why and how the Guardian is counting US police killings. the Guardian. 2017-07-30.
  15. https://mappingpoliceviolence.org/
  16. Durlauf. Steven Neil. Steven Durlauf. Heckman. James Joseph. James Heckman. An Empirical Analysis of Racial Differences in Police Use of Force: A Comment. Journal of Political Economy. 2020-07-21. 128. 10. 3998–4002. University of Chicago. 10.1086/710976. 222811199. 8672021465. 0022-3808. https://archive.today/20201108063306/https://www.journals.uchicago.edu/doi/full/10.1086/710976. 2020-11-08.
  17. Fryer. Roland Gerhard. Roland G. Fryer Jr.. An Empirical Analysis of Racial Differences in Police Use of Force: A Response. Journal of Political Economy. 2020-07-21. 128. 10. 4003–4008. University of Chicago. 10.1086/710977. 222813143. 8672034484. 0022-3808. https://archive.today/20201108065748/https://www.journals.uchicago.edu/doi/full/10.1086/710977. 2020-11-08.
  18. Cesario . Joseph . Johnson . David . Terrill . William . Is There Evidence of Racial Disparity in Police Use of Deadly Force? Analyses of Officer-Involved Fatal Shootings in 2015–2016. . . July 2019 . 10 . 5 . 586–595 . 10.1177/1948550618775108 . 54057840 . November 8, 2020 . November 8, 2020 . https://web.archive.org/web/20201108181622/https://files.osf.io/v1/resources/2btyn/providers/osfstorage/5b263d7bc9cb1b000f4ac380?action=download&direct=&version=1 . dead .
  19. Greenwald . Anthony, G. . Oakes . Mark, A. . Hoffman . Hunter, G. . Targets of discrimination: Effects of race on responses to weapons holders . Journal of Experimental Social Psychology . 11 April 2003 . 39 . 4 . 399–405 . 10.1016/S0022-1031(03)00020-9 .
  20. Correll . Joshua . Wittenbrink . Bernd . Judd . Charles, M. . Park . Bernadette . The Police Officer's Dilemma: Using Ethnicity to Disambiguate Potentially Threatening Individuals . Journal of Personality and Social Psychology . 2002 . 83 . 6 . 1314–1329 . 10.1037/0022-3514.83.6.1314. 12500813 .
  21. Payne . B. Keith . Prejudice and Perception: The Role of Automatic and Controlled Processes in Misperceiving a Weapon . Journal of Personality and Social Psychology . August 2001 . 81 . 2 . 181–192 . 10.1037/0022-3514.81.2.181 . 11519925.
  22. Correll . Joshua . Park . Bernadette . Judd . Charles, M. . Wittenbrink . Bernd . The influence of stereotypes on decisions to shoot . European Journal of Social Psychology . 3 July 2007 . 37 . 6 . 1102–1117 . 10.1002/ejsp.450 .
  23. Ma . Debbie, S. . Correll . Joshua . Target prototypicality moderates racial bias in the decision to shoot . Journal of Experimental Social Psychology . March 2011 . 47 . 2 . 391–396 . 10.1016/j.jesp.2010.11.002 . 12 November 2010.
  24. News: Deadly Force, in Black and White. Sagara. Ryan Gabrielson, Ryann Grochowski Jones, Eric. 2014-10-10. ProPublica. 2017-07-30. en.
  25. Smith. Douglas A.. 1986. The Neighborhood Context of Police Behavior. Crime and Justice. 8. 313–341. 10.1086/449126. 144977273.
  26. Terrill. William. Reisig. Michael D.. 2016-08-18. Neighborhood Context and Police Use of Force. Journal of Research in Crime and Delinquency. en. 40. 3. 291–321. 10.1177/0022427803253800. 43353487.
  27. Ma. Debbie S.. Correll. Joshua. Wittenbrink. Bernd. Bar-Anan. Yoav. Sriram. N.. Nosek. Brian A.. 2013-11-01. When Fatigue Turns Deadly: The Association Between Fatigue and Racial Bias in the Decision to Shoot. Basic and Applied Social Psychology. 35. 6. 515–524. 10.1080/01973533.2013.840630. 29835235.
  28. Correll. Joshua. Park. Bernadette. Judd. Charles M.. Wittenbrink. Bernd. Sadler. Melody S.. Keesee. Tracie. Across the thin blue line: Police officers and racial bias in the decision to shoot. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. en. 92. 6. 1006–1023. 10.1037/0022-3514.92.6.1006. 17547485. 2007. 17992608.
  29. 10.1002/ejsp.450 . The influence of stereotypes on decisions to shoot. European Journal of Social Psychology. 37. 6. 1102–1117. 2007. Correll. Joshua. Park. Bernadette. Judd. Charles M.. Wittenbrink. Bernd.
  30. Correll. Joshua. Wittenbrink. Bernd. Crawford. Matthew T.. Sadler. Melody S.. Stereotypic vision: How stereotypes disambiguate visual stimuli. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. en. 108. 2. 219–233. 10.1037/pspa0000015. 25603373. 2015. free.
  31. Payne. B. Keith. Prejudice and perception: The role of automatic and controlled processes in misperceiving a weapon. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. en. 81. 2. 181–192. 10.1037/0022-3514.81.2.181. 2001. 11519925. 15865607.
  32. Payne. B. Keith. 2006-12-01. Weapon Bias: Split-Second Decisions and Unintended Stereotyping. Current Directions in Psychological Science. en. 15. 6. 287–291. 10.1111/j.1467-8721.2006.00454.x. 16595511. free.
  33. James. Lois. James. Stephen M.. Vila. Bryan J.. 2016-01-14. The Reverse Racism Effect. Criminology & Public Policy. 15. 2. 457–479. 10.1111/1745-9133.12187. 1538-6473.
  34. 10.1016/0047-2352(96)00015-3 . Racial discrimination in the criminal justice system: Findings and problems in the literature. Journal of Criminal Justice. 24. 4. 309–322. 1996. Weitzer. Ronald.
  35. News: Being a cop showed me just how racist and violent the police are. There's only one fix.. Washington Post. 2017-07-30. Hudson . Redditt. 2014-12-06.
  36. News: Michigan police suspend cop behind 'racist' footage of black man. NY Daily News. 2017-07-30. en.
  37. News: Ohio cops suspended for exchanging racist text messages. NY Daily News. 2017-07-30. en.
  38. News: Two Black Cops Write Statements About Racism in LAPD. BET.com. 2017-07-30.
  39. Smith. Brad W.. 2003-05-01. The Impact of Police Officer Diversity on Police-Caused Homicides. Policy Studies Journal. en. 31. 2. 147–162. 10.1111/1541-0072.t01-1-00009. free.
  40. Sim. Jessica J.. Correll. Joshua. Sadler. Melody S.. 2013-03-01. Understanding Police and Expert Performance: When Training Attenuates (vs. Exacerbates) Stereotypic Bias in the Decision to Shoot. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin. en. 39. 3. 291–304. 10.1177/0146167212473157. 23401478. 34947053.
  41. Plant. E. Ashby. Peruche. B. Michelle. Butz. David A.. Eliminating automatic racial bias: Making race non-diagnostic for responses to criminal suspects. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology. 41. 2. 141–156. 10.1016/j.jesp.2004.07.004. 2005.
  42. Web site: How to not shoot civilians: 9 community policing tips from a chief who got it right. Lind. Dara. 2014-09-18. Vox. 2017-07-30.
  43. Weisbuch. Max. Pauker. Kristin. Ambady. Nalini. 2009-12-18. The Subtle Transmission of Race Bias via Televised Nonverbal Behavior. Science. en. 326. 5960. 1711–1714. 10.1126/science.1178358. 3764987. 20019288. 2009Sci...326.1711W.
  44. Weisbuch. Max. Pauker. Kristin. 2011-12-01. The Nonverbal Transmission of Intergroup Bias: A Model of Bias Contagion with Implications for Social Policy. Social Issues and Policy Review. en. 5. 1. 257–291. 10.1111/j.1751-2409.2011.01032.x. 3756600. 23997812.