Monotropism Explained

Monotropism is an individual's tendency to focus their attention on a small or singular number of interests at any time, with them neglecting or not perceiving lesser interests. This cognitive strategy has been posited as the central underlying feature of autism. The theory of monotropism was developed by Dinah Murray, Wenn Lawson and Mike Lesser starting in the 1990s, and first published in 2005.[1] Lawson's further work on the theory formed the basis of his PhD, Single Attention and Associated Cognition in Autism, and book The Passionate Mind published in 2011.

A tendency to focus attention tightly has a number of psychological implications, with it being seen as a state of "tunnel vision". While monotropism tends to cause people to miss things outside their attention tunnel, within it, their focused attention can lend itself to intense experiences, deep thinking, and more specifically, flow states.[2] However, this form of hyperfocus makes it harder to redirect attention, including starting and stopping tasks, leading to what is often described as executive dysfunction in autism, and stereotypes or perseveration, where a person's attention is repeatedly drawn back to the same subject or activity.

Characteristics

Since the amount of attention available to a person is limited, cognitive processes are forced to compete. In the monotropic mind, interests that are active at any given time tend to consume most of the available attention, causing difficulty with other tasks such as conventional social interaction. Language development can be affected, both through the broad attention required and the psychological impact of language, which provides a tool for others to manipulate a child's interest system.[1]

Monotropic individuals have trouble processing multiple things at once, particularly when it comes to multitasking while listening. For example, some students have trouble taking notes in class while listening to a teacher[3] and may find it difficult to read a person's face and comprehend what they are saying simultaneously. A common tendency is for individuals to avoid complex sensory environments because of this hypersensitivity. Monotropic individuals may suppress attention and focus on something else, or develop great depth in a given interest or skill.[4]

Implications for practice

Murray et al. (2005) proposed certain steps to help autistic individuals, such as increasing "connections", building understanding through the child's interests, and making connections between people and concepts more "meaningful and less complex."

See also

Further reading

External links

Notes and References

  1. Murray . Dinah . Dinah Murray . Lesser . Mike . Mike Lesser . Lawson . Wenn . 2005 . Attention, monotropism and the diagnostic criteria for autism . . 9 . 2 . 139–56 . 10.1177/1362361305051398 . 15857859 . 6476917 . free.
  2. Book: Andy . McDonnell . Damian . Milton . 2014 . BILD . 9781905218356 . Jones . Glenys . Birmingham, UK . 38–47 . Going with the flow: reconsidering 'repetitive behaviour' through the concept of 'flow states' . Damian Milton . Hurley . Elizabeth.
  3. Book: Bogdashina, Olga . Sensory Perceptual Issues in Autism and Asperger Syndrome: Different Sensory Experiences – Different Perceptual Worlds . . 2003 . 9781843101666 . London.
  4. Book: Lesser . Mike . Murray . Dinah . . 2020 . 9781912755394 . Murray . Dinah . Dinah Murray . Shoreham by Sea . Mind as a Dynamical System: Implication for Autism . Mike Lesser . Dinah Murray . 1998 . Milton . Damian . Damian Milton . Ridout . Susy . Martin . Nicola . Mills . Richard . https://monotropism.org/dinah/mind-as-a-dynamical-system/.