IQ classification is the practice of categorizing human intelligence, as measured by intelligence quotient (IQ) tests, into categories such as "superior" or "average".
In the current IQ scoring method, an IQ score of 100 means that the test-taker's performance on the test is of average performance in the sample of test-takers of about the same age as was used to norm the test. An IQ score of 115 means performance one standard deviation above the mean, while a score of 85 means performance one standard deviation below the mean, and so on. This "deviation IQ" method is now used for standard scoring of all IQ tests in large part because they allow a consistent definition of IQ for both children and adults. By the current "deviation IQ" definition of IQ test standard scores, about two-thirds of all test-takers obtain scores from 85 to 115, and about 5 percent of the population scores above 125 (i.e. normal distribution).
When IQ testing was first created, Lewis Terman and other early developers of IQ tests noticed that most child IQ scores come out to approximately the same number regardless of testing procedure. Variability in scores can occur when the same individual takes the same test more than once. Further, a minor divergence in scores can be observed when an individual takes tests provided by different publishers at the same age. There is no standard naming or definition scheme employed universally by all test publishers for IQ score classifications.
Even before IQ tests were invented, there were attempts to classify people into intelligence categories by observing their behavior in daily life. Those other forms of behavioral observation were historically important for validating classifications based primarily on IQ test scores. Some early intelligence classifications by IQ testing depended on the definition of "intelligence" used in a particular case. Current IQ test publishers take into account reliability and error of estimation in the classification procedure.
Pupil | KABC-II | WISC-III | WJ-III | |
---|---|---|---|---|
Asher | 90 | 95 | 111 | |
Brianna | 125 | 110 | 105 | |
Colin | 100 | 93 | 101 | |
Danica | 116 | 127 | 118 | |
Elpha | 93 | 105 | 93 | |
Fritz | 106 | 105 | 105 | |
Georgi | 95 | 100 | 90 | |
Hector | 112 | 113 | 103 | |
Imelda | 104 | 96 | 97 | |
Jose | 101 | 99 | 86 | |
Keoku | 81 | 78 | 75 | |
Leo | 116 | 124 | 102 |
IQ tests generally are reliable enough that most people 10 years of age and older have similar IQ scores throughout life. Still, some individuals score very differently when taking the same test at different times or when taking more than one kind of IQ test at the same age. About 42% of children change their score by 5 or more points when re-tested.[1]
For example, many children in the famous longitudinal Genetic Studies of Genius begun in 1921 by Lewis Terman showed declines in IQ as they grew up.Terman recruited school pupils based on referrals from teachers, and gave them his Stanford–Binet IQ test. Children with an IQ above 140 by that test were included in the study. There were 643 children in the main study group. When the students who could be contacted again (503 students) were retested at high school age, they were found to have dropped 9 IQ points on average in Stanford–Binet IQ. Some children dropped by 15 IQ points and or by 25 points or more. Yet parents of those children thought that the children were still as bright as ever, or even brighter.
Because all IQ tests have error of measurement in the test-taker's IQ score, a test-giver should always inform the test-taker of the confidence interval around the score obtained on a given occasion of taking each test. IQ scores are ordinal scores and are not expressed in an interval measurement unit. Besides the reported error interval around IQ test scores, an IQ score could be misleading if a test-giver failed to follow standardized administration and scoring procedures. In cases of test-giver mistakes, the usual result is that tests are scored too leniently, giving the test-taker a higher IQ score than the test-taker's performance justifies. On the other hand, some test-givers err by showing a "halo effect", with low-IQ individuals receiving IQ scores even lower than if standardized procedures were followed, while high-IQ individuals receive inflated IQ scores.
The categories of IQ vary between IQ test publishers as the category labels for IQ score ranges are specific to each brand of test. The test publishers do not have a uniform practice of labeling IQ score ranges, nor do they have a consistent practice of dividing up IQ score ranges into categories of the same size or with the same boundary scores. Thus psychologists should specify which test was given when reporting a test-taker's IQ category if not reporting the raw IQ score. Psychologists and IQ test authors recommend that psychologists adopt the terminology of each test publisher when reporting IQ score ranges.
IQ classifications from IQ testing are not the last word on how a test-taker will do in life, nor are they the only information to be considered for placement in school or job-training programs. There is still a dearth of information about how behavior differs between people with differing IQ scores. For placement in school programs, for medical diagnosis, and for career advising, factors other than IQ can be part of an individual assessment as well.
There are a variety of individually administered IQ tests in use. Not all report test results as "IQ", but most now report a standard score with a mean score level of 100. When a test-taker scores higher or lower than the median score, the score is indicated as 15 standard score points higher or lower for each standard deviation difference higher or lower in the test-taker's performance on the test item content.
See main article: Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale.
See also: Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children and Wechsler Preschool and Primary Scale of Intelligence.
The Wechsler intelligence scales were originally developed from earlier intelligence scales by David Wechsler. David Wechsler, using the clinical and statistical skills he gained under Charles Spearman and as a World War I psychology examiner, crafted a series of intelligence tests. These eventually surpassed other such measures, becoming the most widely used and popular intelligence assessment tools for many years. The first Wechsler test published was the Wechsler–Bellevue Scale in 1939. The Wechsler IQ tests for children and for adults are the most frequently used individual IQ tests in the English-speaking world and in their translated versions are perhaps the most widely used IQ tests worldwide. The Wechsler tests have long been regarded as the "gold standard" in IQ testing. The Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale—Fourth Edition (WAIS–IV) was published in 2008 by The Psychological Corporation. The Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children—Fifth Edition (WISC–V) was published in 2014 by The Psychological Corporation, and the Wechsler Preschool and Primary Scale of Intelligence—Fourth Edition (WPPSI–IV) was published in 2012 by The Psychological Corporation. Like all current IQ tests, the Wechsler tests report a "deviation IQ" as the standard score for the full-scale IQ, with the norming sample mean raw score defined as IQ 100 and a score one standard deviation higher defined as IQ 115 (and one deviation lower defined as IQ 85).
During the First World War in 1917, adult intelligence testing gained prominence as an instrument for assessing drafted soldiers in the United States. Robert Yerkes, an American psychologist, was assigned to devise psychometric tools to allocate recruits to different levels of military service, leading to the development of the Army Alpha and Army Beta group-based tests. The collective efforts of Binet, Simon, Terman, and Yerkes laid the groundwork for modern intelligence test series.[2]
130 and above | Very Superior | |
---|---|---|
120–129 | Superior | |
110–119 | High Average | |
90–109 | Average | |
80–89 | Low Average | |
70–79 | Borderline | |
69 and below | Extremely Low |
130 and above | Extremely High | |
---|---|---|
120–129 | Very High | |
110–119 | High Average | |
90–109 | Average | |
80–89 | Low Average | |
70–79 | Very Low | |
69 and below | Extremely Low |
Psychologists have proposed alternative language for Wechsler IQ classifications. The term "borderline", which implies being very close to being intellectually disabled (defined as IQ under 70), is replaced in the alternative system by a term that doesn't imply a medical diagnosis.
130+ | Very superior | Upper extreme | |
---|---|---|---|
120–129 | Superior | Well above average | |
110–119 | High average | High average | |
90–109 | Average | Average | |
80–89 | Low average | Low average | |
70–79 | Borderline | Well below average | |
69 and below | Extremely low | Lower extreme |
See main article: Stanford–Binet Intelligence Scales.
The current fifth edition of the Stanford–Binet scales (SB5) was developed by Gale H. Roid and published in 2003 by Riverside Publishing. Unlike scoring on previous versions of the Stanford–Binet test, SB5 IQ scoring is deviation scoring in which each standard deviation up or down from the norming sample median score is 15 points from the median score, IQ 100, just like the standard scoring on the Wechsler tests.
140+ | Very gifted or highly advanced | |
---|---|---|
130–140 | Gifted or very advanced | |
120–129 | Superior | |
110–119 | High average | |
90–109 | Average | |
80–89 | Low average | |
70–79 | Borderline impaired or delayed | |
55–69 | Mildly impaired or delayed | |
40–54 | Moderately impaired or delayed |
See main article: Woodcock–Johnson Tests of Cognitive Abilities.
The Woodcock–Johnson a III NU Tests of Cognitive Abilities (WJ III NU) was developed by Richard W. Woodcock, Kevin S. McGrew and Nancy Mather and published in 2007 by Riverside. The WJ III classification terms are not applied.
131 and above | Very superior | |
---|---|---|
121 to 130 | Superior | |
111 to 120 | High Average | |
90 to 110 | Average | |
80 to 89 | Low Average | |
70 to 79 | Low | |
69 and below | Very Low |
The Kaufman Adolescent and Adult Intelligence Test was developed by Alan S. Kaufman and Nadeen L. Kaufman and published in 1993 by American Guidance Service. Kaufman test scores "are classified in a symmetrical, nonevaluative fashion", in other words the score ranges for classification are just as wide above the mean as below the mean, and the classification labels do not purport to assess individuals.
130 and above | Upper Extreme | |
---|---|---|
120–129 | Well Above Average | |
110–119 | Above average | |
90–109 | Average | |
80–89 | Below Average | |
70–79 | Well Below Average | |
69 and below | Lower Extreme |
See main article: Kaufman Assessment Battery for Children.
The Kaufman Assessment Battery for Children, Second Edition was developed by Alan S. Kaufman and Nadeen L. Kaufman and published in 2004 by American Guidance Service.
131–160 | Upper Extreme | |
---|---|---|
116–130 | Above Average | |
85–115 | Average Range | |
70–84 | Below Average | |
40–69 | Lower Extreme |
See main article: Cognitive Assessment System.
The Das-Naglieri Cognitive Assessment System test was developed by Jack Naglieri and J. P. Das and published in 1997 by Riverside.
130 and above | Very Superior | |
---|---|---|
120–129 | Superior | |
110–119 | High Average | |
90–109 | Average | |
80–89 | Low Average | |
70–79 | Below Average | |
69 and below | Well Below Average |
See main article: Differential Ability Scales.
The Differential Ability Scales Second Edition (DAS–II) was developed by Colin D. Elliott and published in 2007 by Psychological Corporation. The DAS-II is a test battery given individually to children, normed for children from ages two years and six months through seventeen years and eleven months. It was normed on 3,480 noninstitutionalized, English-speaking children in that age range. The DAS-II yields a General Conceptual Ability (GCA) score scaled like an IQ score with the mean standard score set at 100 and 15 standard score points for each standard deviation up or down from the mean. The lowest possible GCA score on DAS–II is 30, and the highest is 170.
≥ 130 | Very high | |
---|---|---|
120–129 | High | |
110–119 | Above average | |
90–109 | Average | |
80–89 | Below average | |
70–79 | Low | |
≤ 69 | Very low |
Reynolds Intellectual Ability Scales (RIAS) were developed by Cecil Reynolds and Randy Kamphaus. The RIAS was published in 2003 by Psychological Assessment Resources.
≥ 130 | Significantly above average | |
---|---|---|
120–129 | Moderately above average | |
110–119 | Above average | |
90–109 | Average | |
80–89 | Below average | |
70–79 | Moderately below average | |
≤ 69 | Significantly below average |
Lewis Terman, developer of the Stanford–Binet Intelligence Scales, based his English-language Stanford–Binet IQ test on the French-language Binet–Simon test developed by Alfred Binet. Terman believed his test measured the "general intelligence" construct advocated by Charles Spearman (1904). Terman differed from Binet in reporting scores on his test in the form of intelligence quotient ("mental age" divided by chronological age) scores after the 1912 suggestion of German psychologist William Stern. Terman chose the category names for score levels on the Stanford–Binet test. When he first chose classification for score levels, he relied partly on the usage of earlier authors who wrote, before the existence of IQ tests, on topics such as individuals unable to care for themselves in independent adult life. Terman's first version of the Stanford–Binet was based on norming samples that included only white, American-born subjects, mostly from California, Nevada, and Oregon.
Above 140 | "Near" genius or genius | |
---|---|---|
120–140 | Very superior intelligence | |
110–120 | Superior intelligence | |
90–110 | Normal, or average, intelligence | |
80–90 | Dullness, rarely classifiable as feeble-mindedness | |
70–80 | Border-line deficiency, sometimes classifiable as dullness, often as feeble-mindedness | |
Below 70 | Definite feeble-mindedness |
Rudolph Pintner proposed a set of classification terms in his 1923 book Intelligence Testing: Methods and Results. Pintner commented that psychologists of his era, including Terman, went about "the measurement of an individual's general ability without waiting for an adequate psychological definition." Pintner retained these terms in the 1931 second edition of his book.
130 and above | Very Superior | |
---|---|---|
120–129 | Very Bright | |
110–119 | Bright | |
90–109 | Normal | |
80–89 | Backward | |
70–79 | Borderline |
Albert Julius Levine and Louis Marks proposed a broader set of categories in their 1928 book Testing Intelligence and Achievement. Some of the entries came from contemporary terms for people with intellectual disability.
175 and over | Precocious | |
---|---|---|
150–174 | Very superior | |
125–149 | Superior | |
115–124 | Very bright | |
105–114 | Bright | |
95–104 | Average | |
85–94 | Dull | |
75–84 | Borderline | |
50–74 | Morons | |
25–49 | Imbeciles | |
0–24 | Idiots |
The second revision (1937) of the Stanford–Binet test retained "quotient IQ" scoring, despite earlier criticism of that method of reporting IQ test standard scores. The term "genius" was no longer used for any IQ score range. The second revision was normed only on children and adolescents (no adults), and only "American-born white children".
140 and over | Very superior | |
---|---|---|
120–139 | Superior | |
110–119 | High average | |
90–109 | Normal or average | |
80–89 | Low average | |
70–79 | Borderline defective | |
Below 70 | Mentally defective |
A data table published later as part of the manual for the 1960 Third Revision (Form L-M) of the Stanford–Binet test reported score distributions from the 1937 second revision standardization group.
160–169 | 0.03 | |
---|---|---|
150–159 | 0.2 | |
140–149 | 1.1 | |
130–139 | 3.1 | |
120–129 | 8.2 | |
110–119 | 18.1 | |
100–109 | 23.5 | |
90–99 | 23.0 | |
80–89 | 14.5 | |
70–79 | 5.6 | |
60–69 | 2.0 | |
50–59 | 0.4 | |
40–49 | 0.2 | |
30–39 | 0.03 |
David Wechsler, developer of the Wechsler–Bellevue Scale of 1939 (which was later developed into the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale) popularized the use of "deviation IQs" as standard scores of IQ tests rather than the "quotient IQs" ("mental age" divided by "chronological age") then used for the Stanford–Binet test. He devoted a whole chapter in his book The Measurement of Adult Intelligence to the topic of IQ classification and proposed different category names from those used by Lewis Terman. Wechsler also criticized the practice of earlier authors who published IQ classification tables without specifying which IQ test was used to obtain the scores reported in the tables.
128 and over | Very Superior | 2.2 | |
---|---|---|---|
120–127 | Superior | 6.7 | |
111–119 | Bright Normal | 16.1 | |
91–110 | Average | 50.0 | |
80–90 | Dull normal | 16.1 | |
66–79 | Borderline | 6.7 | |
65 and below | Defective | 2.2 |
In 1958, Wechsler published another edition of his book Measurement and Appraisal of Adult Intelligence. He revised his chapter on the topic of IQ classification and commented that "mental age" scores were not a more valid way to score intelligence tests than IQ scores. He continued to use the same classification terms.
128 and over | Very Superior | 2.2 | |
---|---|---|---|
120–127 | Superior | 6.7 | |
111–119 | Bright Normal | 16.1 | |
91–110 | Average | 50.0 | |
80–90 | Dull normal | 16.1 | |
66–79 | Borderline | 6.7 | |
65 and below | Defective | 2.2 |
The third revision (Form L-M) in 1960 of the Stanford–Binet IQ test used the deviation scoring pioneered by David Wechsler. For rough comparability of scores between the second and third revision of the Stanford–Binet test, scoring table author Samuel Pinneau set 100 for the median standard score level and 16 standard score points for each standard deviation above or below that level. The highest score obtainable by direct look-up from the standard scoring tables (based on norms from the 1930s) was IQ 171 at various chronological ages from three years six months (with a test raw score "mental age" of six years and two months) up to age six years and three months (with a test raw score "mental age" of ten years and three months). The classification for Stanford–Binet L-M scores does not include terms such as "exceptionally gifted" and "profoundly gifted" in the test manual itself. David Freides, reviewing the Stanford–Binet Third Revision in 1970 for the Buros Seventh Mental Measurements Yearbook (published in 1972), commented that the test was obsolete by that year.
140 and over | Very superior | |
---|---|---|
120–139 | Superior | |
110–119 | High average | |
90–109 | Normal or average | |
80–89 | Low average | |
70–79 | Borderline defective | |
Below 70 | Mentally defective |
The first edition of the Woodcock–Johnson Tests of Cognitive Abilities was published by Riverside in 1977. The classifications used by the WJ-R Cog were "modern in that they describe levels of performance as opposed to offering a diagnosis."
131 and above | Very superior | |
---|---|---|
121 to 130 | Superior | |
111 to 120 | High Average | |
90 to 110 | Average | |
80 to 89 | Low Average | |
70 to 79 | Low | |
69 and below | Very Low |
The revised version of the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale (the WAIS-R) was developed by David Wechsler and published by Psychological Corporation in 1981. Wechsler changed a few of the boundaries for classification categories and a few of their names compared to the 1958 version of the test. The test's manual included information about how the actual percentage of people in the norming sample scoring at various levels compared to theoretical expectations.
130+ | Very Superior | 2.6 | 2.2 | |
---|---|---|---|---|
120–129 | Superior | 6.9 | 6.7 | |
110–119 | High Average | 16.6 | 16.1 | |
90–109 | Average | 49.1 | 50.0 | |
80–89 | Low Average | 16.1 | 16.1 | |
70–79 | Borderline | 6.4 | 6.7 | |
below 70 | Mentally Retarded | 2.3 | 2.2 |
The Kaufman Assessment Battery for Children (K-ABC) was developed by Alan S. Kaufman and Nadeen L. Kaufman and published in 1983 by American Guidance Service.
130+ | Upper Extreme | 2.3 | 2.2 | |
---|---|---|---|---|
120–129 | Well Above Average | 7.4 | 6.7 | |
110–119 | Above Average | 16.7 | 16.1 | |
90–109 | Average | 49.5 | 50.0 | |
80–89 | Below Average | 16.1 | 16.1 | |
70–79 | Well Below Average | 6.1 | 6.7 | |
below 70 | Lower Extreme | 2.1 | 2.2 |
The fourth revision of the Stanford–Binet scales (S-B IV) was developed by Thorndike, Hagen, and Sattler and published by Riverside Publishing in 1986. It retained the deviation scoring of the third revision with each standard deviation from the mean being defined as a 16 IQ point difference. The S-B IV adopted new classification terminology. After this test was published, psychologist Nathan Brody lamented that IQ tests had still not caught up with advances in research on human intelligence during the twentieth century.
132 and above | Very superior | |
---|---|---|
121–131 | Superior | |
111–120 | High average | |
89–110 | Average | |
79–88 | Low average | |
68–78 | Slow learner | |
67 or below | Mentally retarded |
The third edition of the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale (WAIS-III) used different classification terminology from the earliest versions of Wechsler tests.
130 and above | Very superior | |
---|---|---|
120–129 | Superior | |
110–119 | High average | |
90–109 | Average | |
80–89 | Low average | |
70–79 | Borderline | |
69 and below | Extremely low |
See main article: Intellectual disability.
See also: Borderline intellectual functioning.
The earliest terms for classifying individuals of low intelligence were medical or legal terms that preceded the development of IQ testing. The legal system recognized a concept of some individuals being so cognitively impaired that they were not responsible for criminal behavior. Medical doctors sometimes encountered adult patients who could not live independently, being unable to take care of their own daily living needs. Various terms were used to attempt to classify individuals with varying degrees of intellectual disability. Many of the earliest terms are now considered extremely offensive.
In current medical diagnosis, IQ scores alone are not conclusive for a finding of intellectual disability. Recently adopted diagnostic standards place the major emphasis on the adaptive behavior of each individual, with IQ score a factor in diagnosis in addition to adaptive behavior scales. Some advocate for no category of intellectual disability to be defined primarily by IQ scores. Psychologists point out that evidence from IQ testing should always be used with other assessment evidence in mind: "In the end, any and all interpretations of test performance gain diagnostic meaning when they are corroborated by other data sources and when they are empirically or logically related to the area or areas of difficulty specified in the referral."
In the United States, the Supreme Court ruled in the case Atkins v. Virginia, 536 U.S. 304 (2002) that states could not impose capital punishment on people with "mental retardation", defined in subsequent cases as people with IQ scores below 70. This legal standard continues to be actively litigated in capital cases.
Historically, terms for intellectual disability eventually became perceived as an insult, in a process commonly known as the euphemism treadmill.[4] [5] [6] The terms mental retardation and mentally retarded became popular in the middle of the 20th century to replace the previous set of terms, which included "imbecile", "idiot", "feeble-minded", and "moron",[7] among others. By the end of the 20th century, retardation and retard became widely seen as disparaging and politically incorrect, although they are still used in some clinical contexts.[8]
The American Association for the Study of the Feeble-minded divided adults with intellectual deficits into three categories. Idiot indicated the greatest degree of intellectual disability in which a person's mental age is below three years. Imbecile indicated an intellectual disability less severe than idiocy and a mental age between three and seven years. Moron was defined as someone a mental age between eight and twelve.[9] Alternative definitions of these terms based on IQ were also used.
The term cretin dates to 1770–80 and comes from a dialectal French word for Christian.[10] The implication was that people with significant intellectual or developmental disabilities were "still human" (or "still Christian") and deserved to be treated with basic human dignity. Although cretin is no longer in use, the term cretinism is still used to refer to the mental and physical disability resulting from untreated congenital hypothyroidism.
Mongolism and Mongoloid idiot were terms used to identify someone with Down syndrome, as the doctor who first described the syndrome, John Langdon Down, believed that children with Down syndrome shared facial similarities with the now-obsolete category of "Mongolian race". The Mongolian People's Republic requested that the medical community cease the use of the term; in 1960, the World Health Organization agreed the term should cease being used.[11]
Retarded comes from the Latin, 'to make slow, delay, keep back, or hinder', so mental retardation meant the same as mentally delayed. The first record of retarded in relation to being mentally slow was in 1895. The term mentally retarded was used to replace terms like idiot, moron, and imbecile because retarded was not then a derogatory term. By the 1960s, however, the term had taken on a partially derogatory meaning. The noun retard is particularly seen as pejorative; a BBC survey in 2003 ranked it as the most offensive disability-related word.[12] The terms mentally retarded and mental retardation are still fairly common, but organizations such as the Special Olympics and Best Buddies are striving to eliminate their use and often refer to retard and its variants as the "r-word". These efforts resulted in U.S. federal legislation, known as Rosa's Law, which replaced the term mentally retarded with the term intellectual disability in federal law.[13] [14]
See main article: Genius.
Francis Galton (1822–1911) was a pioneer in investigating both eminent human achievement and mental testing. In his book Hereditary Genius, written before the development of IQ testing, he proposed that hereditary influences on eminent achievement are strong, and that eminence is rare in the general population. Lewis Terman chose near' genius or genius" as the classification label for the highest classification on his 1916 version of the Stanford–Binet test. By 1926, Terman began publishing about a longitudinal study of California schoolchildren who were referred for IQ testing by their schoolteachers, called Genetic Studies of Genius, which he conducted for the rest of his life. Catherine M. Cox, a colleague of Terman's, wrote a whole book, The Early Mental Traits of 300 Geniuses, published as volume 2 of The Genetic Studies of Genius book series, in which she analyzed biographical data about historic geniuses. Although her estimates of childhood IQ scores of historical figures who never took IQ tests have been criticized on methodological grounds, Cox's study was thorough in finding out what else matters besides IQ in becoming a genius. By the 1937 second revision of the Stanford–Binet test, Terman no longer used the term "genius" as an IQ classification, nor has any subsequent IQ test. In 1939, Wechsler wrote "we are rather hesitant about calling a person a genius on the basis of a single intelligence test score."
The Terman longitudinal study in California eventually provided historical evidence on how genius is related to IQ scores. Many California pupils were recommended for the study by schoolteachers. Two pupils who were tested but rejected for inclusion in the study because of IQ scores too low for the study grew up to be Nobel Prize winners in physics: William Shockley and Luis Walter Alvarez. Based on the historical findings of the Terman study and on biographical examples such as Richard Feynman, who had an IQ of 125 and went on to win the Nobel Prize in physics and become widely known as a genius, the current view of psychologists and other scholars of genius is that a minimum IQ, about 125, is strictly necessary for genius, but that IQ is sufficient for the development of genius only when combined with the other influences identified by Cox's biographical study: an opportunity for talent development along with the characteristics of drive and persistence. Charles Spearman, bearing in mind the influential theory that he originated—that intelligence comprises both a "general factor" and "special factors" more specific to particular mental tasks—wrote in 1927, "Every normal man, woman, and child is, then, a genius at something, as well as an idiot at something."
See main article: Intellectual giftedness.
A major point of consensus among all scholars of intellectual giftedness is that there is no generally agreed upon definition of giftedness. Although there is no scholarly agreement about identifying gifted learners, there is a de facto reliance on IQ scores for identifying participants in school gifted education programs. In practice, many school districts in the United States use an IQ score of 130, including roughly the upper 2 to 3 percent of the national population as a cut-off score for inclusion in school gifted programs.
Five levels of giftedness have been suggested to differentiate the vast difference in abilities that exists between children on varying ends of the gifted spectrum. Although there is no strong consensus on the validity of these quantifiers, they are accepted by many experts of gifted children.
Mildly gifted | 115–129 | +1.00–+1.99 | 1:6–1:44 |
---|---|---|---|
Moderately gifted | 130–144 | +2.00–+2.99 | 1:44–1:1,000 |
Highly gifted | 145–159 | +3.00–+3.99 | 1:1,000–1:10,000 |
Exceptionally gifted | 160–179 | +4.00–+5.33 | 1:10,000–1:1,000,000 |
Profoundly gifted | 180– | +5.33– | < 1:1,000,000 |
As long ago as 1937, Lewis Terman pointed out that error of estimation in IQ scoring increases as IQ score increases, so that there is less and less certainty about assigning a test-taker to one band of scores or another as one looks at higher bands. Current IQ tests also have large error bands for high IQ scores. As an underlying reality, such distinctions as those between "exceptionally gifted" and "profoundly gifted" have never been well established. All longitudinal studies of IQ have shown that test-takers can bounce up and down in score, and thus switch up and down in rank order as compared to one another, over the course of childhood. IQ classification categories such as "profoundly gifted" are those based on the obsolete Stanford–Binet Third Revision (Form L-M) test. The highest reported standard score for most IQ tests is IQ 160, approximately the 99.997th percentile. IQ scores above this level have wider error ranges as there are fewer normative cases at this level of intelligence. Moreover, there has never been any validation of the Stanford–Binet L-M on adult populations, and there is no trace of such terminology in the writings of Lewis Terman. Although two current tests attempt to provide "extended norms" that allow for classification of different levels of giftedness, those norms are not based on well validated data.