Eye movement explained

Eye movement includes the voluntary or involuntary movement of the eyes. Eye movements are used by a number of organisms (e.g. primates, rodents, flies, birds, fish, cats, crabs, octopus) to fixate, inspect and track visual objects of interests. A special type of eye movement, rapid eye movement, occurs during REM sleep.

The eyes are the visual organs of the human body, and move using a system of six muscles. The retina, a specialised type of tissue containing photoreceptors, senses light. These specialised cells convert light into electrochemical signals. These signals travel along the optic nerve fibers to the brain, where they are interpreted as vision in the visual cortex.

Primates and many other vertebrates use three types of voluntary eye movement to track objects of interest: smooth pursuit, vergence shifts[1] and saccades.[2] These types of movements appear to be initiated by a small cortical region in the brain's frontal lobe.[3] [4] This is corroborated by removal of the frontal lobe. In this case, the reflexes (such as reflex shifting the eyes to a moving light) are intact, though the voluntary control is obliterated.[5]

Anatomy

Muscles

See main article: Extraocular muscles. Six extraocular muscles facilitate eye movement. These muscles arise from the common tendinous ring (annulus of Zinn) in the orbit (eye cavity), and attach to the eyeball. The six muscles are the lateral, medial, inferior and superior recti muscles, and the inferior and superior oblique muscles. The muscles, when contracting, cause movement of the eyeball, by pulling the eyeball towards the muscle. For example, the lateral rectus is on the lateral side of the eyeball. When it contracts, the eyeball moves so that the pupil looks outwards. The medial rectus causes the eyeball to look inwards; the inferior rectus downwards and outwards, and the superior rectus upwards and outwards. The superior oblique muscle and inferior oblique muscle attach at angles to the eyeball. The superior oblique muscle moves the eye downwards and inwards whereas the inferior oblique muscle moves the eye upwards and outwards.

Three antagonistic pairs of muscles control eye movement: the lateral and medial recti muscles, the superior and inferior recti muscles, and the superior and inferior oblique muscles. These muscles are responsible for movement of the eye along three different axes: horizontal, either toward the nose (adduction) or away from the nose (abduction); vertical, either elevation or depression; and torsional, movements that bring the top of the eye toward the nose (intorsion) or away from the nose (extorsion). Horizontal movement is controlled entirely by the medial and lateral recti muscles; the medial rectus muscle is responsible for adduction, the lateral rectus muscle for abduction. Vertical movement requires the coordinated action of the superior and inferior recti muscles, as well as the oblique muscles. The relative contribution of the recti and oblique groups depends on the horizontal position of the eye. In the primary position (eyes straight ahead), both of these groups contribute to vertical movement. Elevation is due to the action of the superior rectus and inferior oblique muscles, while depression is due to the action of the inferior rectus and superior oblique muscles. When the eye is abducted, the recti muscles are the prime vertical movers. Elevation is due to the action of the superior rectus, and depression is due to the action of the inferior rectus. When the eye is adducted, the oblique muscles are the prime vertical movers. Elevation is due to the action of the inferior oblique muscle, while depression is due to the action of the superior oblique muscle. The oblique muscles are also primarily responsible for torsional movement.

The muscles are supplied by the oculomotor nerve, with the exception of the superior oblique, which is supplied by the trochlear nerve, and the lateral rectus, supplied by the abducens nerve.[6]

Neuroanatomy

The brain exerts ultimate control over both voluntary and involuntary eye movement. Three cranial nerves carry signals from the brain to control the extraocular muscles. These are the oculomotor nerve, which controls the majority of the muscles, the trochlear nerve, which controls the superior oblique muscle, and the abducens nerve, which controls the lateral rectus muscle.

In addition to the movement of muscles, numerous areas in the brain contribute to involuntary and voluntary eye movement. These include providing the conscious perception of vision, as well as areas that facilitate tracking.

Physiology

Eye movement can be classified according to two systems:

Vergence movement or convergence is the movement of both eyes to make sure that the image of the object being looked at falls on the corresponding spot on both retinas. This type of movement helps in the depth perception of objects[10]

Pursuit movement or smooth pursuit is the movement the eyes make while tracking an object's movement, so that its moving image can remain maintained on fovea.[10]

Saccades

See main article: Saccade. The eyes are never completely at rest: they make frequent fixational eye movement even when fixated at one point. The reason for this movement is related to the photoreceptors and the ganglion cells. It appears that a constant visual stimulus can make the photoreceptors or the ganglion cells become unresponsive; on the other hand a changing stimulus will not. So the eye movement constantly changes the stimuli that fall on the photoreceptors and the ganglion cells, making the image clearer.[10]

Saccades are the rapid movement of eyes that is used while scanning a visual scene. In our subjective impression, the eyes do not move smoothly across the printed page during reading. Instead, they make short and rapid movements called saccades.[11] During each saccade the eyes move as fast as they can and the speed cannot be consciously controlled in between the fixations.[10] Each movement is worth a few minutes of arc, at regular intervals about three to four per second. One of the main uses for saccades is to scan a greater area with the high-resolution fovea of the eye.[12] Research conducted by the University of South Australia in partnership with the University of Stuttgart has revealed the relationship between eye moment and personality traits that AI can read.[13]

Vestibulo-ocular system

See main article: Vestibulo-ocular reflex.

The visual system in the brain is too slow to process that information if the images are slipping across the retina at more than a few degrees per second.[14] Thus, to be able to see while we are moving, the brain must compensate for the motion of the head by turning the eyes. Another specialisation of visual system in many vertebrate animals is the development of a small area of the retina with a very high visual acuity. This area is called the fovea, and covers about 2 degrees of visual angle in people. To get a clear view of the world, the brain must turn the eyes so that the image of the object of regard falls on the fovea. Eye movement is thus very important for visual perception, and any failure can lead to serious visual disabilities. To see a quick demonstration of this fact, try the following experiment: hold your hand up, about one foot (30 cm) in front of your nose. Keep your head still, and shake your hand from side to side, slowly at first, and then faster and faster. At first you will be able to see your fingers quite clearly. But as the frequency of shaking passes about 1 Hz, the fingers will become a blur. Now, keep your hand still, and shake your head (up and down or left and right). No matter how fast you shake your head, the image of your fingers remains clear. This demonstrates that the brain can move the eyes opposite to head motion much better than it can follow, or pursue, a hand movement. When your pursuit system fails to keep up with the moving hand, images slip on the retina and you see a blurred hand.

The brain must point both eyes accurately enough that the object of regard falls on corresponding points of the two retinas to avoid the perception of double vision. In most vertebrates (humans, mammals, reptiles, birds), the movement of different body parts is controlled by striated muscles acting around joints. The movement of the eye is slightly different in that the eyes are not rigidly attached to anything, but are held in the orbit by six extraocular muscles.

Reading

See main article: Eye movement in reading. When reading, the eye moves continuously along a line of text, but makes short rapid movements (saccades) intermingled with short stops (fixations). There is considerable variability in fixations (the point at which a saccade jumps to) and saccades between readers and even for the same person reading a single passage of text.

Music reading

See main article: Eye movement in music reading. Eye movement in music reading is the scanning of a musical score by a musician's eyes. This usually occurs as the music is read during performance, although musicians sometimes scan music silently to study it, and sometimes perform from memory without score. Eye movement in music reading may at first appear to be similar to that in language reading, since in both activities the eyes move over the page in fixations and saccades, picking up and processing coded meanings. However, music is nonlinguistic and involves a strict and continuous time constraint on an output that is generated by a continuous stream of coded instructions.

Scene viewing

Eye movement in scene viewing refers to the visual processing of information presented in scenes. A core aspect of studies in this area is the division of eye movements into the rapid movement of the eyes (saccades), and the focus of the eyes on a point (fixations). Several factors can influence eye movement in scene viewing, including the task and knowledge of the viewer (top-down factors), and the properties of the image being viewed (bottom-up factors).Typically, when presented with a scene, viewers demonstrate short fixation durations and long saccade amplitudes in the earlier phases of viewing an image. This is followed by longer fixations and shorter saccades in the latter phases of scene viewing processing.[15] It has also been found that eye movement behaviour in scene viewing differs with levels of cognitive development - fixation durations are thought to shorten and saccade amplitudes lengthen with an increase in age.[16]

Spatial variation

Where eye movements fixate is affected by both bottom-up and top-down factors. Even an initial glimpse of a scene has an influence on subsequent eye movements.[17] In bottom-up factors, the local contrast or prominence of features in an image,[18] such as a large contrast in luminance[19] or a greater density of edges,[20] can affect the guidance of eye movements.However, the top-down factors of scenes have a greater impact in where eyes fixate. Areas containing more meaningful features,[21] or areas where colour aids the discrimination of objects, can influence eye movements.[22] Images which are related to previous images shown can also have an effect.[23] Eye movements can also be guided towards items when they are heard verbally at the same time as seeing them.[24] Cross-culturally, it has been found that Westerners have an inclination to concentrate on focal objects in a scene, whereas East Asians attend more to contextual information.[25]

Temporal variation

Average fixation durations last for about 330 ms, although there is a large variability in this approximation.[26] This variability is mostly due to the properties of an image and in the task being carried out, which impact both bottom-up and top-down processing.The masking of an image[27] and other degradations, such as a decrease in luminance, during fixations (factors which affect bottom-up processing), have been found to increase the length of fixation durations.[28] However, an enhancement of the image with these factors also increases fixation durations.[29] Factors which affect top-down processing (e.g. blurring) have been found to both increase and decrease fixation durations.[30]

Disorders

Symptoms

Cause

Selected disorders

Vision therapy

See main article: Vision therapy.

In psychotherapy

See main article: Eye movement desensitization and reprocessing.

Terminology

The following terms may be used to describe eye movement:

  1. Dextroversion / right gaze
  2. Laevoversion / left gaze
  3. Sursumversion / elevation / up gaze
  4. Deorsumversion / depression / down gaze
  5. Dextroelevation / gaze up and right
  6. Dextrodepression / gaze down and right
  7. Laevoelevation / gaze up and left
  8. Laevodepression / gaze down and left
  9. Dextrocycloversion – top of the eye rotates to the right
  10. Laevocycloversion – top of the eye rotates to the left

See also

References

External links

Notes and References

  1. Pierrot-Deseilligny. Charles . Milea, D. . Muri, R. M.. 18569409 . Eye movement control by the cerebral cortex. Current Opinion in Neurology. 2004. 17. 17–25. 10.1097/00019052-200402000-00005. 15090873. 1.
  2. Krauzlis RJ . April 2005 . The control of voluntary eye movements: new perspectives . The Neuroscientist . 11 . 2 . 124–37 . 15746381 . 10.1177/1073858404271196 . 10.1.1.135.8577 . 1439113 . 18 February 2006 . https://web.archive.org/web/20060717140143/http://www.snl-k.salk.edu/publications/Krauzlis%20(2005).pdf . 17 July 2006 . dead .
  3. Heinen SJ, Liu M . Sep–Oct 1997 . Single-neuron activity in the dorsomedial frontal cortex during smooth-pursuit eye movements to predictable target motion . Vis Neurosci . 14 . 5. 853–65 . 9364724 . 10.1017/s0952523800011597.
  4. 10760550 . 32 . 2–3 . Eye fields in the frontal lobes of primates . April 2000 . Tehovnik EJ, Sommer MA, Chou IH, Slocum WM, Schiller PH . Brain Res. . 413–48 . 10.1016/s0165-0173(99)00092-2. 10161/11752 . 4467996 . free .
  5. "Sensory Reception: Human Vision: Structure and function of the Human Eye" Encyclopædia Britannica, 1987
  6. Purves D, Augustine GJ, Fitzpatrick D, et al., editors. Neuroscience. 2nd edition. Sunderland (MA): Sinauer Associates; 2001. The Actions and Innervation of Extraocular Muscles. Available from: https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/books/NBK10793/
  7. Robinson FR, Fuchs AF . 14413503 . 2001 . The role of the cerebellum in voluntary eye movements . Annu Rev Neurosci . 24 . 981–1004 . 11520925 . 10.1146/annurev.neuro.24.1.981.
  8. Kanski, JJ. Clinical Ophthalmology: A Systematic Approach. Boston:Butterworth-Heinemann;1989.
  9. Awwad, S. "Motility & Binocular Vision" . EyeWeb.org.
  10. Carlson and Heth (2010). Psychology the Science of Behaviour 4e. Pearson Education Canada. Page 140
  11. Wayne S. Murray. Behavioral and Brain Sciences(2003)26, page 446
  12. John Findlay Saccadic eye movement programming: sensory and attentional factors, Psychological Research (March 2009), 73 (2), pg. 127–135
  13. News: Artificial intelligence can predict your personality ... simply by tracking your eyes. ScienceDaily. 2018-08-02. en.
  14. 10.1364/josa.65.000847 . 1142031 . July 1975. Visual acuity in the presence of retinal-image motion . Journal of the Optical Society of America . 65 . 7 . 847–50 . Westheimer . Gerald . McKee . Suzanne P. . 1975JOSA...65..847W .
  15. Pannasch S. . Helmert J. . Roth K. . Herbold A.-K. . Walter H. . 2008 . Visual Fixation Durations and Saccade Amplitudes: Shifting Relationship in a Variety of Conditions . Journal of Eye Movement Research . 2 . 2 . 1–19 . 10.16910/jemr.2.2.4 . free .
  16. Helo A. . Pannasch S. . Sirri L. . Rämä P. . 2014 . The maturation of eye movement behaviour: scene viewing characteristics in children and adults . Vision Research . 103 . 83–91 . 10.1016/j.visres.2014.08.006 . 25152319. free .
  17. Castelhano M. . Henderson J. . 2007 . Initial Scene Representations Facilitate Eye Movement Guidance in Visual Search . Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance . 33 . 4. 753–763 . 10.1037/0096-1523.33.4.753. 17683226 . 10.1.1.703.1791 .
  18. Itti L. . Koch C. . 2000 . A saliency-based search mechanism for overt and covert shifts of visual attention . Vision Research . 40 . 10–12. 1489–1506 . 10.1016/s0042-6989(99)00163-7. 10788654 . 10.1.1.501.1921 . 192077 .
  19. Parkhurst D. J. . Law K. . Niebur E. . 2002 . Modeling the role of salience in the allocation of overt visual attention . Vision Research . 42 . 1. 107–123 . 10.1016/s0042-6989(01)00250-4. 11804636 . 11780536 . free .
  20. Mannan S. . Ruddock K. . Wooding D. . 1996 . The relationship between the locations of spatial features and those of fixations made during visual examination of briefly presented images . Spatial Vision . 10 . 3. 165–188 . 10.1163/156856896x00123. 9061830 .
  21. Onat S. . Açik A. . Schumann F. . König P. . 2014 . The Contributions of Image Content and Behavioural Relevancy to Overt Attention . PLOS One. 9 . 4. e93254 . 10.1371/journal.pone.0093254. 24736751 . 3988016 . 2014PLoSO...993254O . free .
  22. Amano K. . Foster D. . 2014 . Influence of local scene color on fixation position in visual search . Journal of the Optical Society of America A . 31 . 4. A254–A261 . 10.1364/josaa.31.00a254. 24695179 . 10.1.1.708.7682 . 2014JOSAA..31A.254A .
  23. Henderson J. . Weeks Jr. . Hollingworth A. . 1999 . The Effects of Semantic Consistency on Eye Movements During Complex Scene Viewing . Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance . 25 . 210–228 . 10.1037/0096-1523.25.1.210.
  24. Staub A. . Abbott M. . Bogartz R. . 2012 . Linguistically guided anticipatory eye movements in scene viewing . Visual Cognition . 20 . 8. 922–946 . 10.1080/13506285.2012.715599. 40478177 .
  25. Chua H. . Boland J. . Nisbett R. . 2002 . Cultural variation in eye movement during scene perception . 10.1073/pnas.0506162102. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America . 102 . 35. 12629–12633 . 16116075 . 1194960. free .
  26. Henderson J . 2003 . Human gaze control during real-world scene perception . Trends in Cognitive Sciences . 7 . 11. 498–504 . 10.1016/j.tics.2003.09.006. 14585447 . 10.1.1.545.5406 . 3117689 .
  27. Henderson J. . Pierce G. . 2008 . Eye movements during scene viewing: Evidence for mixed control of fixation durations . Psychonomic Bulletin & Review . 15 . 3. 566–573 . 10.3758/pbr.15.3.566. 18567256 . free .
  28. Henderson J. . Nuthmann A. . Luke S. . 2417324 . 2013 . Eye Movement Control During Scene Viewing: Immediate Effects of Scene Luminance on Fixation Durations . Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance . 39 . 2. 318–322 . 10.1037/a0031224. 23276111 .
  29. Walshe R. . Nuthmann A. . 2014 . Asymmetrical control of fixation durations in scene viewing . 10.1016/j.visres.2014.03.012 . 24726565 . Vision Research . 100 . 38–46 . free . 20.500.11820/226a90e0-144c-4f39-afcc-92fc78506657 . free .
  30. Henderson J. . Olejarczyk J. . Luke S. . Schmidt J. . 2014 . Eye movement control during scene viewing: Immediate degradation and enhancement effects of spatial frequency filtering . Visual Cognition . 22 . 3–4. 486–502 . 10.1080/13506285.2014.897662. 145540524 .
  31. 1. http://www.utdol.com/online/content/topic.do?topicKey=neuro_op/2892&selectedTitle=1~150&source=search_result