Color blindness explained

Color blindness
Synonyms:Color vision deficiency, impaired color vision
Field:Ophthalmology
Symptoms:Decreased ability to see colors
Duration:Long term
Causes:Genetic (inherited usually X-linked)
Diagnosis:Ishihara color test
Treatment:Adjustments to teaching methods, mobile apps
Frequency:Red–green: 8% males, 0.5% females (Northern European descent)

Color blindness or color vision deficiency (CVD) is the decreased ability to see color or differences in color. The severity of color blindness ranges from mostly unnoticeable to full absence of color perception. Color blindness is usually an inherited problem or variation in the functionality of one or more of the three classes of cone cells in the retina, which mediate color vision. The most common form is caused by a genetic condition called congenital red–green color blindness (including protan and deutan types), which affects up to 1 in 12 males (8%) and 1 in 200 females (0.5%).[1] The condition is more prevalent in males, because the opsin genes responsible are located on the X chromosome. Rarer genetic conditions causing color blindness include congenital blue–yellow color blindness (tritan type), blue cone monochromacy, and achromatopsia. Color blindness can also result from physical or chemical damage to the eye, the optic nerve, parts of the brain, or from medication toxicity. Color vision also naturally degrades in old age.

Diagnosis of color blindness is usually done with a color vision test, such as the Ishihara test. There is no cure for most causes of color blindness, however there is ongoing research into gene therapy for some severe conditions causing color blindness. Minor forms of color blindness do not significantly affect daily life and the color blind automatically develop adaptations and coping mechanisms to compensate for the deficiency. However, diagnosis may allow an individual, or their parents/teachers, to actively accommodate the condition.[2] Color blind glasses (e.g. EnChroma) may help the red–green color blind at some color tasks, but they do not grant the wearer "normal color vision" or the ability to see "new" colors.[3] Some mobile apps can use a device's camera to identify colors.[4]

Depending on the jurisdiction, the color blind are ineligible for certain careers,[2] such as aircraft pilots, train drivers, police officers, firefighters, and members of the armed forces.[2] [5] The effect of color blindness on artistic ability is controversial,[2] [6] but a number of famous artists are believed to have been color blind.[2] [7]

Effects

A color blind person will have decreased (or no) color discrimination along the red–green axis, blue–yellow axis, or both. However, the vast majority of the color blind are only affected on their red–green axis.

The first indication of color blindness generally consists of a person using the wrong color for an object, such as when painting, or calling a color by the wrong name. The colors that are confused are very consistent among people with the same type of color blindness.

Confusion colors

Confusion colors are pairs or groups of colors that will often be mistaken by the color blind. Confusion colors for red–green color blindness include:

Confusion colors for tritan include:

These colors of confusion are defined quantitatively by straight confusion lines plotted in CIEXYZ, usually plotted on the corresponding chromaticity diagram. The lines all intersect at a copunctal point, which varies with the type of color blindness.[8] Chromaticities along a confusion line will appear metameric to dichromats of that type. Anomalous trichromats of that type will see the chromaticities as metameric if they are close enough, depending on the strength of their CVD. For two colors on a confusion line to be metameric, the chromaticities first have to be made isoluminant, meaning equal in lightness. Also, colors that may be isoluminant to the standard observer may not be isoluminant to a person with dichromacy.

Color tasks

See main article: Color task. Cole describes four color tasks, all of which are impeded to some degree by color blindness:[9]

The following sections describe specific color tasks with which the color blind typically have difficulty.

Food

Color blindness causes difficulty with the connotative color tasks associated with selecting or preparing food. Selecting food for ripeness can be difficult; the green–yellow transition of bananas is particularly hard to identify. It can also be difficult to detect bruises, mold, or rot on some foods, to determine when meat is done by color, to distinguish some varietals, such as a Braeburn vs. a Granny Smith apple, or to distinguish colors associated with artificial flavors (e.g. jelly beans, sports drinks).

Skin color

Changes in skin color due to bruising, sunburn, rashes or even blushing are easily missed by the red–green color blind.

Traffic lights

The colors of traffic lights can be difficult for the red–green color blindness. This difficulty includes distinguishing red/amber lights from sodium street lamps, distinguishing green lights (closer to cyan) from normal white lights, and distinguishing red from amber lights, especially when there are no positional clues available (see image).

The main coping mechanism to overcome these challenges is to memorize the position of lights. The order of the common triplet traffic light is standardized as red–amber–green from top to bottom or left to right. Cases that deviate from this standard are rare. One such case is a traffic light in Tipperary Hill in Syracuse, New York, which is upside-down (green–amber–red top to bottom) due to the sentiments of its Irish American community.[10] However, the light has been criticized due to the potential hazard it poses for color blind drivers.[11]

There are other several features of traffic lights available that help accommodate the color blind. British Rail signals use more easily identifiable colors: The red is blood red, the amber is yellow and the green is a bluish color. Most British road traffic lights are mounted vertically on a black rectangle with a white border (forming a "sighting board"), so that drivers can more easily look for the position of the light. In the eastern provinces of Canada, traffic lights are sometimes differentiated by shape in addition to color: square for red, diamond for yellow, and circle for green (see image).

Signal lights

Navigation lights in marine and aviation settings employ red and green lights to signal the relative position of other ships or aircraft. Railway signal lights also rely heavily on red–green–yellow colors. In both cases, these color combinations can be difficult for the red–green color blind. Lantern Tests are a common means of simulating these light sources to determine not necessarily whether someone is color blind, but whether they can functionally distinguish these specific signal colors. Those who cannot pass this test are generally completely restricted from working on aircraft, ships or rail, for example.

Fashion

See also: Color of clothing. Color analysis is the analysis of color in its use in fashion, to determine personal color combinations that are most aesthetically pleasing.[12] Colors to combine can include clothing, accessories, makeup, hair color, skin color, eye color, etc. Color analysis involves many aesthetic and comparative color task that can be difficult for the color blind.

Art

Inability to distinguish color does not necessarily preclude the ability to become a celebrated artist. The 20th century expressionist painter Clifton Pugh, three-time winner of Australia's Archibald Prize, on biographical, gene inheritance and other grounds has been identified as a person with protanopia.[13] 19th century French artist Charles Méryon became successful by concentrating on etching rather than painting after he was diagnosed as having a red–green deficiency.[14] Jin Kim's red–green color blindness did not stop him from becoming first an animator and later a character designer with Walt Disney Animation Studios.[15]

Advantages

Deuteranomals are better at distinguishing shades of khaki,[16] which may be advantageous when looking for predators, food, or camouflaged objects hidden among foliage.[17] Dichromats tend to learn to use texture and shape clues and so may be able to penetrate camouflage that has been designed to deceive individuals with normal color vision.[18]

Some tentative evidence finds that the color blind are better at penetrating certain color camouflages. Such findings may give an evolutionary reason for the high rate of red–green color blindness.[19] There is also a study suggesting that people with some types of color blindness can distinguish colors that people with normal color vision are not able to distinguish.[20] In World War II, color blind observers were used to penetrate camouflage.[21]

In the presence of chromatic noise, the color blind are more capable of seeing a luminous signal, as long as the chromatic noise appears metameric to them.[22] This is the effect behind most "reverse" Pseudoisochromatic plates (e.g. "hidden digit" Ishihara plates) that are discernible to the color blind but unreadable to people with typical color vision.

Digital design

See also: Color coding in data visualization. Color codes are useful tools for designers to convey information. The interpretation of this information requires users to perform a variety of Color Tasks, usually comparative but also sometimes connotative or denotative. However, these tasks are often problematic for the color blind when design of the color code has not followed best practices for accessibility.[23] For example, one of the most ubiquitous connotative color codes is the "red means bad and green means good" or similar systems, based on the classic signal light colors. However, this color coding will almost always be undifferentiable to deutans or protans, and therefore should be avoided or supplemented with a parallel connotative system (symbols, smileys, etc.).

Good practices to ensure design is accessible to the color blind include:

Unordered Information

A common task for designers is to select a subset of colors (qualitative colormap) that are as mutually differentiable as possible (salient). For example, player pieces in a board game should be as different as possible.

Classic advice suggests using Brewer palettes, but several of these are not actually accessible to the color blind.

Unfortunately, the colors with the greatest contrast to the red–green color blind tend to be colors of confusion to the blue–yellow color blind, and vice versa. However, since red–green is much more prevalent than blue–yellow CVD, design should generally prioritize those users (deutans then protans).

Ordered Information

A common task for data visualization is to represent a color scale, or sequential colormap, often in the form of a heat map or choropleth. Several scales are designed with special consideration for the color blind and are widespread in academia, including Cividis, Viridis[25] and Parula. These comprise a light-to-dark scale superimposed on a yellow-to-blue scale, making them monotonic and perceptually uniform to all forms of color vision.

Classification

Much terminology has existed and does exist for the classification of color blindness, but the typical classification for color blindness follows the von Kries classifications,[26] which uses severity and affected cone for naming.

Based on severity

Based on clinical appearance, color blindness may be described as total or partial. Total color blindness (monochromacy) is much less common than partial color blindness.[27] Partial color blindness includes dichromacy and anomalous trichromacy, but is often clinically defined as mild, moderate or strong.

Monochromacy

See main article: Monochromacy. Monochromacy is often called total color blindness since there is no ability to see color. Although the term may refer to acquired disorders such as cerebral achromatopsia, it typically refers to congenital color vision disorders, namely rod monochromacy and blue cone monochromacy).[28] [29]

In cerebral achromatopsia, a person cannot perceive colors even though the eyes are capable of distinguishing them. Some sources do not consider these to be true color blindness, because the failure is of perception, not of vision. They are forms of visual agnosia.[29]

Monochromacy is the condition of possessing only a single channel for conveying information about color. Monochromats are unable to distinguish any colors and perceive only variations in brightness. Congenital monochromacy occurs in two primary forms:

  1. Rod monochromacy, frequently called complete achromatopsia, where the retina contains no cone cells, so that in addition to the absence of color discrimination, vision in lights of normal intensity is difficult.
  2. Cone monochromacy is the condition of having only a single class of cone. A cone monochromat can have good pattern vision at normal daylight levels, but will not be able to distinguish hues. Cone monochromacy is divided into classes defined by the single remaining cone class. However, red and green cone monochromats have not been definitively described in the literature. Blue cone monochromacy is caused by lack of functionality of L (red) and M (green) cones, and is therefore mediated by the same genes as red–green color blindness (on the X chromosome). Peak spectral sensitivities are in the blue region of the visible spectrum (near 440 nm). People with this condition generally show nystagmus ("jiggling eyes"), photophobia (light sensitivity), reduced visual acuity, and myopia (nearsightedness).[30] Visual acuity usually falls to the 20/50 to 20/400 range.

Dichromacy

See main article: Dichromacy. Dichromats can match any color they see with some mixture of just two primary colors (in contrast to those with normal sight (trichromats) who can distinguish three primary colors). Dichromats usually know they have a color vision problem, and it can affect their daily lives. Dichromacy in humans includes protanopia, deuteranopia, and tritanopia. Out of the male population, 2% have severe difficulties distinguishing between red, orange, yellow, and green (orange and yellow are different combinations of red and green light). Colors in this range, which appear very different to a normal viewer, appear to a dichromat to be the same or a similar color. The terms protanopia, deuteranopia, and tritanopia come from Greek, and respectively mean "inability to see (anopia) with the first (prot-), second (deuter-), or third (trit-) [cone]".

Anomalous trichromacy

Anomalous trichromacy is the mildest type of color deficiency, but the severity ranges from almost dichromacy (strong) to almost normal trichromacy (mild).[31] In fact, many mild anomalous trichromats have very little difficulty carrying out tasks that require normal color vision and some may not even be aware that they have a color vision deficiency. The types of anomalous trichromacy include protanomaly, deuteranomaly and tritanomaly. It is approximately three times more common than dichromacy.[32] Anomalous trichromats exhibit trichromacy, but the color matches they make differ from normal trichromats. In order to match a given spectral yellow light, protanomalous observers need more red light in a red/green mixture than a normal observer, and deuteranomalous observers need more green. This difference can be measured by an instrument called an Anomaloscope, where red and green lights are mixed by a subject to match a yellow light.[33]

Based on affected cone

There are two major types of color blindness: difficulty distinguishing between red and green, and difficulty distinguishing between blue and yellow.[34] [35] These definitions are based on the phenotype of the partial color blindness. Clinically, it is more common to use a genotypical definition, which describes which cone/opsin is affected.

Red–green color blindness

Red–green color blindness includes protan and deutan CVD. Protan CVD is related to the L-cone and includes protanomaly (anomalous trichromacy) and protanopia (dichromacy). Deutan CVD is related to the M-cone and includes deuteranomaly (anomalous trichromacy) and deuteranopia (dichromacy).[36] [37] The phenotype (visual experience) of deutans and protans is quite similar. Common colors of confusion include red/brown/green/yellow as well as blue/purple. Both forms are almost always symptomatic of congenital red–green color blindness, so affects males disproportionately more than females.[38] This form of color blindness is sometimes referred to as daltonism after John Dalton, who had red–green dichromacy. In some languages, daltonism is still used to describe red–green color blindness.

Blue–yellow color blindness

Blue–yellow color blindness includes tritan CVD. Tritan CVD is related to the S-cone and includes tritanomaly (anomalous trichromacy) and tritanopia (dichromacy). Blue–yellow color blindness is much less common than red–green color blindness, and more often has acquired causes than genetic. Tritans have difficulty discerning between bluish and greenish hues.[41] Tritans have a neutral point at 571 nm (yellowish).

Summary of cone complements

The below table shows the cone complements for different types of human color vision, including those considered color blindness, normal color vision and 'superior' color vision. The cone complement contains the types of cones (or their opsins) expressed by an individual.

Cone systemRed GreenBlueN = normal
A = anomalous
NANANA
1Normal visionTrichromacyNormal
2ProtanomalyAnomalous trichromacyPartial
color
blindness
Red–
green
3ProtanopiaDichromacy
4DeuteranomalyAnomalous trichromacy
5DeuteranopiaDichromacy
6TritanomalyAnomalous trichromacyBlue–
yellow
7TritanopiaDichromacy
8Blue cone monochromacyMonochromacyTotal color blindness
9Achromatopsia
10Tetrachromacy
(carrier theory)
Tetrachromacy'Superior'

Causes

See also: Trichromatic color vision. Color blindness is any deviation of color vision from normal trichromatic color vision (often as defined by the standard observer) that produces a reduced gamut. Mechanisms for color blindness are related to the functionality of cone cells, and often to the expression of photopsins, the photopigments that 'catch' photons and thereby convert light into chemical signals.

Color vision deficiencies can be classified as inherited or acquired.

Genetics

Color blindness is typically an inherited genetic disorder. The most common forms of color blindness are associated with the Photopsin genes, but the mapping of the human genome has shown there are many causative mutations that do not directly affect the opsins. Mutations capable of causing color blindness originate from at least 19 different chromosomes and 56 different genes (as shown online at the Online Mendelian Inheritance in Man [OMIM]).

Genetics of red–green color blindness

By far the most common form of color blindness is congenital red–green color blindness (Daltonism), which includes protanopia/protanomaly and deuteranopia/deuteranomaly. These conditions are mediated by the OPN1LW and OPN1MW genes, respectively, both on the X chromosome. An 'affected' gene is either missing (as in Protanopia and Deuteranopia - Dichromacy) or is a chimeric gene (as in Protanomaly and Deuteranomaly).

Since the OPN1LW and OPN1MW genes are on the X chromosome, they are sex-linked, and therefore affect males and females disproportionately. Because the color blind 'affected' alleles are recessive, color blindness specifically follows X-linked recessive inheritance. Males have only one X chromosome (XY), and females have two (XX); Because the male only has one of each gene, if it is affected, the male will be color blind. Because a female has two alleles of each gene (one on each chromosome), if only one gene is affected, the dominant normal alleles will "override" the affected, recessive allele and the female will have normal color vision. However, if the female has two mutated alleles, she will still be color blind. This is why there is a disproportionate prevalence of color blindness, with ~8% of males exhibiting color blindness and ~0.5% of females.

Genetics of blue–yellow color blindness

Congenital blue–yellow color blindness is a much rarer form of color blindness including tritanopia/tritanomaly. These conditions are mediated by the OPN1SW gene on Chromosome 7 which encodes the S-opsin protein and follows autosomal dominant inheritance. The cause of blue–yellow color blindness is not analogous to the cause of red–green color blindness, i.e. the peak sensitivity of the S-opsin does not shift to longer wavelengths. Rather, there are 6 known point mutations of OPN1SW that degrade the performance of the S-cones.[45] The OPN1SW gene is almost invariant in the human population. Congenital tritan defects are often progressive, with nearly normal trichromatic vision in childhood (e.g. mild tritanomaly) progressing to dichromacy (tritanopia) as the S-cones slowly die.[45] Tritanomaly and tritanopia are therefore different penetrance of the same disease, and some sources have argued that tritanomaly therefore be referred to as incomplete tritanopia.[46]

Other genetic causes

Several inherited diseases are known to cause color blindness, including achromatopsia, cone dystrophy, Leber's congenital amaurosis and retinitis pigmentosa. These can be congenital or commence in childhood or adulthood. They can be static/stationary or progressive. Progressive diseases often involve deterioration of the retina and other parts of the eye, so often progress from color blindness to more severe visual impairments, up to and including total blindness.

Non-genetic causes

Physical trauma can cause color blindness, either neurologically – brain trauma which produces swelling of the brain in the occipital lobe – or retinally, either acute (e.g. from laser exposure) or chronic (e.g. from ultraviolet light exposure).

Color blindness may also present itself as a symptom of degenerative diseases of the eye, such as cataract and age-related macular degeneration, and as part of the retinal damage caused by diabetes. Vitamin A deficiency may also cause color blindness.[47]

Color blindness may be a side effect of prescription drug use. For example, red–green color blindness can be caused by ethambutol, a drug used in the treatment of tuberculosis.[48] Blue–yellow color blindness can be caused by sildenafil, an active component of Viagra.[49] Hydroxychloroquine can also lead to hydroxychloroquine retinopathy, which includes various color defects.[50] Exposure to chemicals such as styrene[51] or organic solvents[52] [53] can also lead to color vision defects.

Simple colored filters can also create mild color vision deficiencies. John Dalton's original hypothesis for his deuteranopia was actually that the vitreous humor of his eye was discolored:

An autopsy of his eye after his death in 1844 showed this to be definitively untrue,[54] though other filters are possible. Actual physiological examples usually affect the blue–yellow opponent channel and are named Cyanopsia and Xanthopsia, and are most typically an effect of yellowing or removal of the lens.

The opponent channels can also be affected by the prevalence of certain cones in the retinal mosaic. The cones are not equally prevalent and not evenly distributed in the retina. When the number of one of these cone types is significantly reduced, this can also lead to or contribute to a color vision deficiency. This is one of the causes of tritanomaly.

Some people are also unable to distinct between blue and green, which appears to be a combination of culture and exposure to UV-light.[55]

Diagnosis

Color vision test

See main article: Color vision test. The main method for diagnosing a color vision deficiency is in testing the color vision directly. The Ishihara color test is the test most often used to detect red–green deficiencies and most often recognized by the public.[2] Some tests are clinical in nature, designed to be fast, simple, and effective at identifying broad categories of color blindness. Others focus on precision and are generally available only in academic settings.[56]

Genetic testing

While genetic testing cannot directly evaluate a subject's color vision (phenotype), most congenital color vision deficiencies are well-correlated with genotype. Therefore, the genotype can be directly evaluated and used to predict the phenotype. This is especially useful for progressive forms that do not have a strongly color deficient phenotype at a young age. However, it can also be used to sequence the L- and M-Opsins on the X-chromosome, since the most common alleles of these two genes are known and have even been related to exact spectral sensitivities and peak wavelengths. A subject's color vision can therefore be classified through genetic testing,[58] but this is just a prediction of the phenotype, since color vision can be affected by countless non-genetic factors such as your cone mosaic.

Management

Despite much recent improvement in gene therapy for color blindness, there is currently no FDA approved treatment for any form of CVD, and otherwise no cure for CVD currently exists. Management of the condition by using lenses to alleviate symptoms or smartphone apps to aid with daily tasks is possible.

Lenses

See main article: Color blind glasses.

There are three kinds of lenses that an individual can wear that can increase their accuracy in some color related tasks (although none of these will "fix" color blindness or grant the wearer normal color vision):

Aids

Many mobile and computer applications have been developed to aid color blind individuals in completing color tasks:

In 2003, a cybernetic device called eyeborg was developed to allow the wearer to hear sounds representing different colors.[67] Achromatopsic artist Neil Harbisson was the first to use such a device in early 2004; the eyeborg allowed him to start painting in color by memorizing the sound corresponding to each color. In 2012, at a TED Conference, Harbisson explained how he could now perceive colors outside the ability of human vision.[68]

Epidemiology

Rates of color blindness
Males Females
Dichromacy 2.4% 0.03%
Protanopia 1.3% 0.02%
Deuteranopia 1.2% 0.01%
Tritanopia 0.008% 0.008%
Anomalous trichromacy 6.3% 0.37%
Protanomaly 1.3% 0.02%
Deuteranomaly 5.0% 0.35%
Tritanomaly 0.0001% 0.0001%
Color blindness affects a large number of individuals, with protans and deutans being the most common types.[36] In individuals with Northern European ancestry, as many as 8 percent of men and 0.4 percent of women experience congenital color deficiency.[69] [70] Interestingly, even Dalton's first paper already arrived upon this 8% number:

History

During the 17th and 18th century, several philosophers hypothesized that not all individuals perceived colors in the same way:[71]

Gordon Lynn Walls claims[72] that the first well-circulated case study of color blindness was published in a 1777 letter from Joseph Huddart to Joseph Priestley, which described "Harris the Shoemaker" and several of his brothers with what would later be described as protanopia. There appear to be no earlier surviving historical mentions of color blindness, despite its prevalence.[72]

The phenomenon only came to be scientifically studied in 1794, when English chemist John Dalton gave the first account of color blindness in a paper to the Manchester Literary and Philosophical Society, which was published in 1798 as Extraordinary Facts relating to the Vision of Colours: With Observations.[73] Genetic analysis of Dalton's preserved eyeball confirmed him as having deuteranopia in 1995, some 150 years after his death.[74]

Influenced by Dalton, German writer J. W. von Goethe studied color vision abnormalities in 1798 by asking two young subjects to match pairs of colors.

In 1837, August Seebeck first discriminated between protans and deutans (then as class I + II).[75] [72] He was also the first to develop an objective test method, where subjects sorted colored sheets of paper, and was the first to describe a female colorblind subject.[76]

In 1875, the Lagerlunda train crash in Sweden brought color blindness to the forefront. Following the crash, Professor Alarik Frithiof Holmgren, a physiologist, investigated and concluded that the color blindness of the engineer (who had died) had caused the crash. Professor Holmgren then created the first test for color vision using multicolored skeins of wool to detect color blindness and thereby exclude the color blind from jobs in the transportation industry requiring color vision to interpret safety signals.[77] However, there is a claim that there is no firm evidence that color deficiency did cause the collision, or that it might have not been the sole cause.[78]

In 1920, Frederick William Edridge-Green devised an alternative theory of color vision and color blindness based on Newton's classification of 7 fundamental colors (ROYGBIV). Edridge-Green classified color vision based on how many distinct colors a subject could see in the spectrum. Normal subjects were termed hexachromic as they could not discern Indigo. Subjects with superior color vision, who could discern indigo, were heptachromic. The color blind were therefore dichromic (equivalent to dichromacy) or tri-, tetra- or pentachromic (anomalous trichromacy).[79] [80]

Rights

In the United States, under federal anti-discrimination laws such as the Americans with Disabilities Act, color vision deficiencies have not been found to constitute a disability that triggers protection from workplace discrimination.

A Brazilian court ruled that the color blind are protected by the Inter-American Convention on the Elimination of All Forms of Discrimination against Person with Disabilities.[81] [82] [83] At trial, it was decided that the carriers of color blindness have a right of access to wider knowledge, or the full enjoyment of their human condition.

Occupations

Color blindness may make it difficult or impossible for a person to engage in certain activities. Persons with color blindness may be legally or practically barred from occupations in which color perception is an essential part of the job (e.g., mixing paint colors), or in which color perception is important for safety (e.g., operating vehicles in response to color-coded signals). This occupational safety principle originates from the aftermath of the 1875 Lagerlunda train crash, which Alarik Frithiof Holmgren blamed on the color blindness of the engineer and created the first occupational screening test (Holmgren's wool test) against the color blind.[77]

Color vision is important for occupations using telephone or computer networking cabling, as the individual wires inside the cables are color-coded using green, orange, brown, blue and white colors.[84] Electronic wiring, transformers, resistors, and capacitors are color-coded as well, using black, brown, red, orange, yellow, green, blue, violet, gray, white, silver, and gold.[85]

Participation, officiating and viewing sporting events can be impacted by color blindness. Professional football players Thomas Delaney and Fabio Carvalho have discussed the difficulties when color clashes occur, and research undertaken by FIFA has shown that enjoyment and player progression can be hampered by issues distinguishing the difference between the pitch and training objects or field markings.[86] Snooker World Champions Mark Williams and Peter Ebdon sometimes need to ask the referee for help distinguishing between the red and brown balls due to their color blindness. Both have played foul shots on notable occasions by the wrong ball.[87] [88] [89]

Driving

Red–green color blindness can make it difficult to drive, primarily due to the inability to differentiate red–amber–green traffic lights. Protans are further disadvantaged due to the darkened perception of reds, which can make it more difficult to quickly recognize brake lights.[90] In response, some countries have refused to grant driver's licenses to individuals with color blindness:

Piloting aircraft

Although many aspects of aviation depend on color coding, only a few of them are critical enough to be interfered with by some milder types of color blindness. Some examples include color-gun signaling of aircraft that have lost radio communication, color-coded glide-path indications on runways, and the like. Some jurisdictions restrict the issuance of pilot credentials to persons with color blindness for this reason. Restrictions may be partial, allowing color-blind persons to obtain certification but with restrictions, or total, in which case color-blind persons are not permitted to obtain piloting credentials at all.[97]

In the United States, the Federal Aviation Administration requires that pilots be tested for normal color vision as part of their medical clearance in order to obtain the required medical certificate, a prerequisite to obtaining a pilot's certification. If testing reveals color blindness, the applicant may be issued a license with restrictions, such as no night flying and no flying by color signals—such a restriction effectively prevents a pilot from holding certain flying occupations, such as that of an airline pilot, although commercial pilot certification is still possible, and there are a few flying occupations that do not require night flight and thus are still available to those with restrictions due to color blindness (e.g., agricultural aviation). The government allows several types of tests, including medical standard tests (e.g., the Ishihara, Dvorine, and others) and specialized tests oriented specifically to the needs of aviation. If an applicant fails the standard tests, they will receive a restriction on their medical certificate that states: "Not valid for night flying or by color signal control". They may apply to the FAA to take a specialized test, administered by the FAA. Typically, this test is the "color vision light gun test". For this test an FAA inspector will meet the pilot at an airport with an operating control tower. The color signal light gun will be shone at the pilot from the tower, and they must identify the color. If they pass they may be issued a waiver, which states that the color vision test is no longer required during medical examinations. They will then receive a new medical certificate with the restriction removed. This was once a Statement of Demonstrated Ability (SODA), but the SODA was dropped, and converted to a simple waiver (letter) early in the 2000s.[98]

Research published in 2009 carried out by the City University of London's Applied Vision Research Centre, sponsored by the UK's Civil Aviation Authority and the U.S. Federal Aviation Administration, has established a more accurate assessment of color deficiencies in pilot applicants' red/green and yellow–blue color range which could lead to a 35% reduction in the number of prospective pilots who fail to meet the minimum medical threshold.[99]

See also

Further reading

External links

Notes and References

  1. Web site: 2017-10-18 . Colour vision deficiency (colour blindness) . 2022-03-17 . nhs.uk . en.
  2. Gordon N . Colour blindness . Public Health . 112 . 2 . 81–4 . March 1998 . 9581449 . 10.1038/sj.ph.1900446 . 0033-3506.
  3. Gómez-Robledo . L . Do EnChroma glasses improve color vision for colorblind subjects? . Optics Express . 2018 . 26 . 22 . 28693–28703 . 10.1364/OE.26.028693. 30470042 . 2018OExpr..2628693G . 53721875 . free . 10481/57698 . free .
  4. Web site: Facts About Color Blindness. NEI. 29 July 2016. February 2015. live. https://web.archive.org/web/20160728003639/https://nei.nih.gov/health/color_blindness/facts_about. 28 July 2016.
  5. Web site: OSHA does not have requirements for normal color vision. Occupational Safety and Health Administration. www.osha.gov. 2019-05-06.
  6. Marmor MF, Lanthony P . The dilemma of color deficiency and art . Survey of Ophthalmology . 45 . 5 . 407–15 . March 2001 . 11274694 . 10.1016/S0039-6257(00)00192-2 .
  7. Marmor MF . Vision, eye disease, and art: 2015 Keeler Lecture . Eye . 30 . 2 . 287–303 . February 2016 . 26563659 . 4763116 . 10.1038/eye.2015.197 .
  8. Fomins . S . Multispectral analysis of color vision deficiency tests . Materials Science . 2011 . 17 . 1 . 104–108 . 10.5755/j01.ms.17.1.259. free .
  9. Cole . Barry L . The handicap of abnormal colour vision . Clinical and Experimental Optometry . 1972 . 55 . 8 . 304–310 . 10.1111/j.1444-0938.1972.tb06271.x.
  10. News: New documentary uncovers the Irish links to America's Tipperary Hill. 15 August 2017. TheJournal.ie. 6 November 2016. live. https://web.archive.org/web/20170815182810/http://www.thejournal.ie/tipperary-hill-radio-documentary-3063059-Nov2016/. 15 August 2017.
  11. Web site: The Story Behind Syracuse's Upside-Down Traffic Light. Zhang, Sarah. Gizmodo. 17 March 2014 . live. https://web.archive.org/web/20140916034424/http://gizmodo.com/the-story-behind-syracuses-upside-down-traffic-light-1545301615. 2014-09-16.
  12. Web site: 2022-01-01 . What is Color Analysis? . 2024-02-21 . London Image Institute . en-US.
  13. Cole BL, Harris RW . Colour blindness does not preclude fame as an artist: celebrated Australian artist Clifton Pugh was a protanope . Clinical & Experimental Optometry . 92 . 5 . 421–8 . September 2009 . 19515095 . 10.1111/j.1444-0938.2009.00384.x . 21676461 . free .
  14. Encyclopedia: Charles Meryon. Anon. Art Encyclopedia. The Concise Grove Dictionary of Art.. Oxford University Press. 7 January 2010. live. https://web.archive.org/web/20101125063246/http://www.answers.com/topic/charles-m-ryon. 25 November 2010.
  15. News: Lee . Hyo-won . Dreams come true, Disney style . 25 November 2019 . The Korea Times . 15 May 2011.
  16. News: 10.1038/news051205-1. Colour blindness may have hidden advantages. Nature. December 5, 2005. Simonite. Tom .
  17. Bosten . J.M. . Robinson . J.D. . Jordan . G. . Mollon . J.D. . Multidimensional scaling reveals a color dimension unique to 'color-deficient' observers . Current Biology . December 2005 . 15 . 23 . R950–R952 . 10.1016/j.cub.2005.11.031. 16332521 . 6966946 . free . 2005CBio...15.R950B .
  18. 10.1038/146226a0 . Colour-Blindness and Camouflage . Nature . 1940 . 146 . 3694 . 226 . 1940Natur.146Q.226. . 4071103 . free .
  19. Morgan MJ, Adam A, Mollon JD . 35694740 . Dichromats detect colour-camouflaged objects that are not detected by trichromats . Proceedings. Biological Sciences . 248 . 1323 . 291–5 . June 1992 . 1354367 . 10.1098/rspb.1992.0074 . 1992RSPSB.248..291M .
  20. Bosten JM, Robinson JD, Jordan G, Mollon JD . Multidimensional scaling reveals a color dimension unique to 'color-deficient' observers . Current Biology . 15 . 23 . R950-2 . December 2005 . 16332521 . 10.1016/j.cub.2005.11.031 . 6966946 . free . 2005CBio...15.R950B .
  21. News: Colour blindness not all it seems. 21 June 2016. BBC News. 6 December 2015. live. https://web.archive.org/web/20160623215610/http://news.bbc.co.uk/2/hi/health/4498734.stm. 23 June 2016.
  22. Sousa . Bruna Rafaela Silva . Loureiro . Terezinha Medeiros Gonçalves . Goulart . Paulo Roney Kilpp . Cortes . Maria Izabel Tentes . Costa . Marcelo Fernandes . Bonci . Daniela Maria Oliveira . Baran . Luiz Claudio Portnoi . Hauzman . Einat . Ventura . Dora Fix . Miquilini . Leticia . Souza . Givago Silva . Specificity of the chromatic noise influence on the luminance contrast discrimination to the color vision phenotype . Scientific Reports . 21 October 2020 . 10 . 1 . 17897 . 10.1038/s41598-020-74875-3. 33087826 . 7578001 . 2020NatSR..1017897S .
  23. Hovis . Jeffery K. . July 2002 . Diagnosis of Defective Colour Vision, 2nd Ed. . Optometry and Vision Science . en-US . 79 . 7 . 406 . 10.1097/00006324-200207000-00005 . 1538-9235. free .
  24. Book: Caprette. Heather. Best Practices in Accessible Online Design. Pressbooks @ MSL. 14 Avoiding the Use of Color Alone to Convey Meaning and Algorithms That Help. https://pressbooks.ulib.csuohio.edu/accessibility/chapter/chapter-2-3-avoiding-the-use-of-color-alone-to-convey-meaning-and-algorithms-that-help/.
  25. Nuñez . JR . 2018 . Optimizing colormaps with consideration for color vision deficiency to enable accurate interpretation of scientific data . PLOS ONE . 13 . 7 . e0199239 . 10.1371/journal.pone.0199239 . 30067751 . 6070163 . 1712.01662 . 2018PLoSO..1399239N . free .
  26. von Kries . J. . 1897 . Ueber Farbensysteme . Zeitschrift für Psychologie Physiologie Sinnesorg . 13 . 241–324.
  27. Web site: Human Vision and Color Perception . . Spring . Kenneth R. . Matthew J. . Parry-Hill . Thomas J. . Fellers . Michael W. . Davidson . 2007-04-05 . live . https://web.archive.org/web/20070827191749/http://micro.magnet.fsu.edu/primer/lightandcolor/humanvisionhome.html . 2007-08-27 .
  28. Web site: Types of Colour Blindness. Colour Blind Awareness. live. https://web.archive.org/web/20140529052207/http://www.colourblindawareness.org/colour-blindness/types-of-colour-blindness/. 2014-05-29.
  29. Book: Blom, Jan Dirk . A Dictionary of Hallucinations . Springer . 2009 . 978-1-4419-1222-0 . 4 . live . https://web.archive.org/web/20161227164302/https://books.google.com/books?id=KJtQptBcZloC&pg=PA4 . 2016-12-27 .
  30. Weiss AH, Biersdorf WR . Blue cone monochromatism . Journal of Pediatric Ophthalmology and Strabismus . 26 . 5 . 218–23 . 1989 . 10.3928/0191-3913-19890901-04 . 2795409 . 23037026 .
  31. Simunovic MP . Colour vision deficiency . Eye . 24 . 5 . 747–55 . May 2010 . 19927164 . 10.1038/eye.2009.251 . free .
  32. 10.1016/j.gde.2006.04.002. Genetics of variation in human color vision and the retinal cone mosaic. 2006. Deeb. Samir S.. Current Opinion in Genetics & Development. 16. 3. 301–307. 16647849.
  33. 10.1016/0042-6989(79)90209-8. Optimization of a Rayleigh-type equation for the detection of tritanomaly. 1979. Moreland. J.D.. Kerr. J.. Vision Research. 19. 12. 1369–1375. 316945. 29379397 .
  34. Web site: Accommodating Color Blindness . Paul S. . Hoffman . 2009-07-01 . dead . https://web.archive.org/web/20080515103432/http://www.digitalspaceart.com/articles/ColorBlindness.pdf . 15 May 2008 .
  35. Web site: Severity of Colorblindness Varies . Neitz . Maureen E. . Maureen Neitz . . 2007-04-05 . dead . https://web.archive.org/web/20070205055320/http://healthlink.mcw.edu/article/999211295.html . 5 February 2007 .
  36. Wong . B. . June 2011 . Color blindness . Nature Methods . 8 . 6 . 441 . 36690778 . 21774112 . 10.1038/nmeth.1618.
  37. Neitz J, Neitz M . The genetics of normal and defective color vision . Vision Research . 51 . 7 . 633–51 . April 2011 . 21167193 . 3075382 . 10.1016/j.visres.2010.12.002 .
  38. Book: Harrison. G.A.. Tanner. J.M.. Pilbeam. D.R.. Baker. P.T.. Human Biology. 183–187, 287–290. Oxford. Oxford University Press. 1988. 978-0-19-854144-8.
  39. Neitz. Jay. Maureen. Neitz. The genetics of normal and defective color vision. Vision Research. 51. 7. 2011. 633–651. 10.1016/j.visres.2010.12.002. 21167193. 3075382.
  40. Book: Contributions to color science . MacAdam . David L. . Judd . Deane B. . NBS . 1979 . 584 .
  41. Steefel, Lorraine T., and Timothy E. Moore, PhD. "Color Blindness." The Gale Encyclopedia of Nursing and Allied Health, edited by Jacqueline L. Longe, 4th ed., vol. 2, Gale, 2018, pp. 890–892. Gale eBooks, Accessed 29 Dec. 2021.
  42. Web site: Disease-causing Mutations and protein structure . https://web.archive.org/web/20050501081119/http://www.biochem.ucl.ac.uk/bsm/humgen/chr__034.html#304000 . dead . 2005-05-01 . UCL Biochemistry BSM Group . 2007-04-02 .
  43. V . Ionica . Gastaud . P . Test chromatique pour dépistage et étalonnage des dyschromatopsies . Color vision test for detection and evaluation of dyschromatopsia . Journal Français d'Ophtalmologie . fr . 19 . 11 . 9033889.
  44. Web site: Acquired colour vision defects . colourblindawareness.org . https://web.archive.org/web/20141216094549/http://www.colourblindawareness.org/colour-blindness/acquired-colour-vision-defects/ . 2014-12-16.
  45. Book: Rodriguez-Carmona . Marisa . Patterson . Emily J. . Photoreceptors, Color Vision . Encyclopedia of Color Science and Technology . 2020 . 1–7 . 10.1007/978-3-642-27851-8_277-3. 978-3-642-27851-8 . 226504635 . https://openaccess.city.ac.uk/id/eprint/23584/1/Photoreceptors%20Color%20Vision_submitted%20to%20CRO.pdf .
  46. Book: Sharpe . LT . Stockman . A . Jägle . H . Nathans . J . Color vision: From genes to perception . 1999 . 351 . Opsin genes, cone photopigments, color vision, and color blindness. . True cases of tritanomaly, as distinct from partial or incomplete tritanopia, have never been satisfactorily documented. Although the separate existence of tritanopia and tritanomaly, with different modes of inheritance, has been postulated, it now seems more likely that tritanomaly does not exist, but rather has been mistaken for incomplete tritanopia..
  47. Book: Leikin . Jerrold B. . Lipsky . Martin S. . 2003 . Complete Medical Encyclopedia . First . 388 . Random House, for the American Medical Association . New York, NY . Random House Reference . 978-0-8129-9100-0 . archive.org . 1 December 2011 . registration.
  48. Web site: Unknown . Unknown . NEI - Types of Color Vision Deficiency . live . https://web.archive.org/web/20140708223716/http://www.rxlist.com/myambutol-drug.htm . 2014-07-08 . 2024-07-21 . www.nei.nih.gov . Color Blindness, Color Vision Deficiency . Description, Types of Color Vision Deficiency,.
  49. Web site: VIAGRA (SILDENAFIL CITRATE) DRUG . drug information . RxList.com . Description, user reviews, drug side effects, interactions–prescribing information . 2022-06-03.
  50. Book: Fraunfelder . Frederick T. . Fraunfelder . Frederick W. . Chambers . Wiley A. . 2014 . Drug-Induced Ocular Side Effects: Clinical ocular toxicology e‑book . 79 . Elsevier Health Sciences . 978-0-323-31985-0 . en.
  51. Choi AR, Braun JM, Papandonatos GD, Greenberg PB . November 2017 . Occupational styrene exposure and acquired dyschromatopsia: A systematic review and meta-analysis . American Journal of Industrial Medicine . 60 . 11 . 930–946 . 28836685 . 5652067 . 10.1002/ajim.22766.
  52. Betancur-Sánchez AM, Vásquez-Trespalacios EM, Sardi-Correa C . January 2017 . Impaired colour vision in workers exposed to organic solvents: A systematic review . Archivos de la Sociedad Espanola de Oftalmologia . 92 . 1 . 12–18 . 27422480 . 10.1016/j.oftal.2016.05.008.
  53. Dick . F.D. . March 2006 . Solvent neurotoxicity . Occupational and Environmental Medicine . 63 . 3 . 221–6, 179 . 16497867 . 2078137 . 10.1136/oem.2005.022400.
  54. Hunt . D. . Dulai . K. . Bowmaker . J. . Mollon . J. . The Chemistry of John Dalton's Color Blindness . Science . February 17, 1995 . 267 . 5200 . 984–988 . 10.1126/science.7863342. 7863342 . 1995Sci...267..984H . 6764146 .
  55. Josserand . Mathilde . Meeussen . Emma . Majid . Asifa . Dediu . Dan . 2021 . Environment and culture shape both the colour lexicon and the genetics of colour perception . Scientific Reports . en . 11 . 1 . 19095 . 10.1038/s41598-021-98550-3 . 34580373 . 8476573 . 2021NatSR..1119095J . 2045-2322.
  56. Toufeeq A . Specifying colours for colour vision testing using computer graphics . Eye . 18 . 10 . 1001–5 . October 2004 . 15192692 . 10.1038/sj.eye.6701378 . free .
  57. Kinnear PR, Sahraie A . New Farnsworth–Munsell 100 hue test norms of normal observers for each year of age 5–22 and for age decades 30–70 . The British Journal of Ophthalmology . 86 . 12 . 1408–11 . December 2002 . 12446376 . 1771429 . 10.1136/bjo.86.12.1408 .
  58. Web site: Color vision deficiency. Reference. Genetics Home. Genetics Home Reference. en. 2019-05-06.
  59. Siegel IM . The X-Chrom lens. On seeing red . Survey of Ophthalmology . 25 . 5 . 312–24 . 1981 . 6971497 . 10.1016/S0039-6257(81)80001-X .
  60. Zeltzer HI . Use of modified X-Chrom for relief of light dazzlement and color blindness of a rod monochromat . Journal of the American Optometric Association . 50 . 7 . 813–8 . July 1979 . 315420 .
  61. https://artoptical.com/lenses/special-lens-designs/x-chrom/ An X-Chrom manual
  62. Welsh . Kenneth W . Aeromedical implications of the X-chrom lens for improving color vision deficiencies . Aviation, Space, and Environmental Medicine . April 1978 . 50 . 3 . 249–255 . Federal Aviation Administration . Oklahoma City . 313209 . 30 September 2022.
  63. Web site: Zhou. Li. A Scientist Accidentally Developed Sunglasses That Could Correct Color Blindness. Smithsonian. 6 January 2018. en.
  64. Gómez-Robledo . L. . Valero . E. M. . Huertas . R. . Martínez-Domingo . M. A. . Hernández-Andrés . J. . Do EnChroma glasses improve color vision for colorblind subjects? . Optics Express . 29 October 2018 . 26 . 22 . 28693–28703 . 10.1364/OE.26.028693. 30470042 . 2018OExpr..2628693G . 53721875 . free . 10481/57698 . free .
  65. Simon-Liedtke . Joschua Thomas . Farup . Ivar . Evaluating color vision deficiency daltonization methods using a behavioral visual-search method . Journal of Visual Communication and Image Representation . February 2016 . 35 . 236–247 . 10.1016/j.jvcir.2015.12.014. 11250/2461824 . free .
  66. Web site: Colour Blindness: Experience it. Colour Blind Awareness. en-US. 2019-12-11.
  67. Alfredo M. Ronchi: Eculture: Cultural Content in the Digital Age. Springer (New York, 2009). p. 319
  68. http://www.ted.com/talks/neil_harbisson_i_listen_to_color.html "I listen to color"
  69. Birch. Jennifer . Subjects with colour vision deficiency in the community: what do primary care physicians need to know? . Journal of the Optical Society of America A . 29 . 3 . 2012 . 313–320 . 10.1364/JOSAA.29.000313. 22472762 .
  70. Chan . Xin . Goh . Shi . Tan . Ngiap . Subjects with colour vision deficiency in the community: what do primary care physicians need to know? . Asia Pacific Family Medicine . 13 . 1 . 2014 . 10 . 10.1186/s12930-014-0010-3 . free .
  71. Book: Lanthony . Philippe . The History of Color Blindness . 2018 . Wayenborgh Publishing . 978-90-6299-903-3 . 3 . 14 April 2022.
  72. Walls . Gordon L. . The G. Palmer Story (Or, "What It's Like, Sometimes, To Be A Scientist") . Journal of the History of Medicine and Allied Sciences . 1956 . 11 . 1 . 66–96 . 10.1093/jhmas/XI.1.66 . 24619193 . 13295579 . 0022-5045.
  73. Dalton . John . John Dalton . Extraordinary Facts relating to the Vision of Colours: With Observations . Manchester Literary and Philosophical Society . 1798 . 5 . 1 . 28–45 . Memoirs . England, Manchester.
  74. Hunt. D. M. . Dulai . K. S. . Bowmaker . J. K. . Mollon . J. D. . February 17, 1995 . The chemistry of John Dalton's color blindness . Science . 267 . 5200 . 984–988 . 10.1126/science.7863342. 7863342 . 1995Sci...267..984H . 6764146 .
  75. Seebeck . August . Über den bei mancher Personen vorkommenden Mangel an Farbensinn . Annalen der Physik . 1837 . 42.
  76. Lee . BB . The evolution of concepts of color vision. . Neurociencias . 1 July 2008 . 4 . 4 . 209–224 . 21593994. 3095437 .
  77. Vingrys AJ, Cole BL . Origins of colour vision standards within the transport industry . Ophthalmic & Physiological Optics . 6 . 4 . 369–75 . 1986 . 3306566 . 10.1111/j.1475-1313.1986.tb01155.x . 41486427 .
  78. Mollon JD, Cavonius LR . The Lagerlunda collision and the introduction of color vision testing . Survey of Ophthalmology . 57 . 2 . 178–94 . 2012 . 22301271 . 10.1016/j.survophthal.2011.10.003 .
  79. McLaren . K. . Newton's indigo . Color Research & Application . 1985 . 10 . 4 . 225–229 . 10.1002/col.5080100411.
  80. Edridge-Green . F. W. . Trichromic Vision and Anomalous Trichromatism . Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Containing Papers of a Biological Character . 1913 . 86 . 586 . 164–170 . 10.1098/rspb.1913.0010 . 80517 . 129045064 . 0950-1193.
  81. Web site: Full text of the decision of the court – in Portuguese language . 2012-03-09 . dead . https://archive.today/20120714220139/http://pt.scribd.com/doc/65834303/Acordao-Direito-Educacao-Deficientes . 2012-07-14 .
  82. Web site: Decree issued by president of a republic ratifying Legislative Decree No. 198, of june 13, which approved the Inter-American Convention AG/RES. 1608 – in Portuguese language . 2012-03-09 . live . https://web.archive.org/web/20120325074802/http://www.planalto.gov.br/ccivil_03/decreto/2001/D3956.htm . 2012-03-25 .
  83. Web site: Inter-American Convention on the Elimination of All Forms of Discrimination against Person with Disabilities. . 2012-03-09 . live . https://web.archive.org/web/20130416205804/http://www.oas.org/juridico/english/ga-res99/eres1608.htm . 2013-04-16 .
  84. Book: Meyers, Michael. All in One A+ Certification Exam Guide. 4th. 2002. McGraw-Hill/Osborne. Berkeley, California. 978-0-07-222274-6.
  85. Book: Grob, Bernard. Basic Electronics. 2001. Glencoe/McGraw-Hill. Columbus, Ohio. 978-0-02-802253-6.
  86. Web site: Football players shocked when experiencing Colour Blindness. Tacbis.
  87. Web site: Billiard Index: Mark Williams MBE player profile . Billiard Index . 15 November 2023 . dead . https://web.archive.org/web/20080708183938/http://www.billiardindex.com/MarkWilliams.html . 8 July 2008.
  88. Web site: Hawkins on Top Down Under . dead . . 2012-07-15 . 2023-11-15 . 2023-10-17 . https://web.archive.org/web/20231017161334/https://wst.tv/hawkins-on-top-down-under/.
  89. Web site: 13 October 2008 . Rueful Ebdon mistakes brown for red . live . https://web.archive.org/web/20120504082800/http://news.bbc.co.uk/sport2/hi/other_sports/snooker/7668421.stm . 4 May 2012 . 15 November 2023 . BBC Sport.
  90. Cole . Barry . Colour Blindness and Driving . Clinical and Experimental Optometry . September 2016 . 99 . 5 . 484–487 . 10.1111/cxo.12396. 27470192 . 26368283 . free .
  91. Web site: ORDIN 87 03/02/2003 – Portal Legislativ . PORTAL LEGISLATIV . Ministerul Justiției . 31 December 2021. ro.
  92. Web site: ORDIN 87 03/02/2003 – Portal Legislativ . PORTAL LEGISLATIV . Ministerul Justiției . 31 December 2021. ro.
  93. Web site: Corlăţean . Titus . Discrimination against Romanians with genetic chromatic deficiencies . 31 December 2021. en.
  94. News: . Mild to medium colour blind people can now obtain driver's license . . Press Trust of India . 2020-06-26 . 2022-06-01.
  95. Web site: Lu . Feiran . Some of us see the world in a different light . Shine . 1 June 2022.
  96. Web site: Do color blindmen a driver's license. Color blindmen allowed to drive cars . Rozavet . 1 June 2022.
  97. Web site: Answers for Pilots: Color vision. January 2012.
  98. Web site: Aerospace Medical Dispositions – Color vision . 2009-04-11 . live . https://web.archive.org/web/20090512031034/https://www.faa.gov/about/office_org/headquarters_offices/avs/offices/aam/ame/guide/app_process/exam_tech/item52/amd/ . 2009-05-12 .
  99. Web site: Colour-blindness research could clear more pilots to fly: UK CAA. Warburton. Simon. 29 May 2009. Air transport. Reed Business Information. 29 October 2009. live. https://web.archive.org/web/20090602082019/http://www.flightglobal.com/articles/2009/05/29/327137/colour-blindness-research-could-clear-more-pilots-to-fly-uk.html. 2 June 2009.