Biology and political orientation explained

A number of studies have found that human biology can be linked with political orientation.[1] This means that an individual's biology may predispose them to a particular political orientation and ideology or, conversely, that subscription to certain ideologies may predispose them to measurable biological and health outcomes.

Studies have found that subjects with right-wing (or conservative in the United States) political views have larger amygdalae, report larger social networks and greater happiness than liberals, are more prone to express disgust to moral infringements and are more sensitive to perceived threats. Those with left-wing (or liberal in the United States) political views have a larger volume of grey matter in the anterior cingulate cortex and are more likely to report greater relationship dissatisfaction and emotional distress than conservatives, to show more openness to experience as well as greater tolerance for uncertainty and disorder.

Genetic factors account for at least some of the variation of political views.[2] [3] From the perspective of evolutionary psychology, conflicts regarding redistribution of wealth may have been common in the ancestral environment and humans may have developed psychological mechanisms for judging their own chances of succeeding in such conflicts. These mechanisms may affect political views. However, many of the studies linking biology to politics remain controversial and unreplicated.[4] [5]

Brain studies

A 2011 study by cognitive neuroscientist Ryota Kanai at University College London found structural brain differences between subjects of different political orientation in a convenience sample of students at the same college.[6] The researchers performed MRI scans on the brains of 90 volunteer students who had indicated their political orientation on a five-point scale ranging from "very liberal" to "very conservative".[6] [7]

Students who reported more conservative political views were found to have larger amygdala,[6] a structure in the temporal lobes whose primary function is in the formation, consolidation and processing of memory, as well as positive and negative conditioning (emotional learning).[8] The amygdala is responsible for important roles in social interaction, such as the recognition of emotional cues in facial expressions and the monitoring of personal space,[9] [10] with larger amygdalae correlating with larger and more complex social networks.[11] [12] It is also postulated to play a role in threat detection, including modulation of fear and aggression to perceived threats.[13] [14] [15] Conservative students were also found to have greater volume of gray matter in the left insula and the right entorhinal cortex.[6] There is evidence that conservatives are more sensitive to disgust[16] and one role of the insula is in the modulation of social emotions, such as the feeling of disgust to specific sights, smells and norm violations.[17] [18] [19]

Students who reported more liberal political views were found to have a larger volume of grey matter in the anterior cingulate cortex,[6] a structure of the brain associated with emotional awareness and the emotional processing of pain.[20] [21] The anterior cingulate cortex becomes active in situations of uncertainty,[22] and is postulated to play a role in error detection, such as the monitoring and processing of conflicting stimuli or information.[23]

The authors concluded, "Although our data do not determine whether these regions play a causal role in the formation of political attitudes, they converge with previous work to suggest a possible link between brain structure and psychological mechanisms that mediate political attitudes."[6] In an interview with LiveScience, Ryota Kanai said, "It's very unlikely that actual political orientation is directly encoded in these brain regions", and that "more work is needed to determine how these brain structures mediate the formation of political attitude."[1] [7] [24] [25] Kanai and colleagues added that it is necessary to conduct a longitudinal study to determine whether the changes in brain structure that we observed lead to changes in political behavior or whether political attitudes and behavior instead result in changes of brain structure.

Functional differences

Psychometrics

Various studies suggest measurable differences in the psychological traits of liberals and conservatives. Conservatives are more likely to report larger social networks, greater happiness and self-esteem than liberals, are more reactive to perceived threats and more likely to interpret ambiguous facial expressions as threatening.[26] [27] [28] [29] Liberals are more likely to report greater emotional distress, relationship dissatisfaction and experiential hardship than conservatives, and show more openness to experience as well as greater tolerance for uncertainty and disorder.[27] [29] [26]

Behavioral studies

A study by David Amodio et al. at New York University and the University of California, Los Angeles, found differences in how self-described liberal and conservative research participants responded to changes in patterns.[30] Participants were asked to tap a keyboard when the letter "M" appeared on a computer monitor and to refrain from tapping when they saw a "W". The letter "M" appeared four times more frequently than "W", conditioning participants to press the keyboard when a letter appears. Liberal participants made fewer mistakes than conservatives during testing and their electroencephalograph readings showed more activity in the anterior cingulate cortex, the part of the brain that deals with conflicting information, during the experiment, suggesting that they were better able to detect conflicts in established patterns. Amodio warned against concluding that a particular political orientation is superior. He said: "The tendency of conservatives to block distracting information could be a good thing depending on the situation."[31] [32]

A 2017 study both replicated the original study and also found that conservatives performed better in a task in which choosing the simple strategy was the more optimal solution; while both liberals and conservatives started the task attempting the more complex but less effective strategy, conservatives switched to the simple strategy more quickly than liberals.[33] Amodio's original study was also replicated by Weissflog et al. (2013)[34] and Inzlicht et al. (2009).[35] Conversely, Kremláček et al. (2019)[36] and Wendell (2016)[37] did not replicate Amodio's results. Both studies also argued that Weissflog and Inzlicht's results were not as concordant with the Amodio's findings as originally claimed.

A study of subjects' reported level of disgust linked to various scenarios showed that people who scored highly on the "disgust sensitivity" scale held more politically conservative views,[16] which some researchers believe could be partially explained by personality traits.[38] However, the findings of a 2019 study suggest that sensitivity to disgust among conservatives varies according to the elicitors used, and that using an elicitor-unspecific scale caused the differences in sensitivity to disappear between those of different political orientations.[39]

A 2018 study in the United States looking at levels of cognitive reflection (the tendency to favour analytic reasoning over instinctive or "gut" responses) found that those who voted for Donald Trump in the 2016 US presidential election had lower levels of cognitive reflection than Hillary Clinton voters or third-party voters. However, this effect was mostly driven by Democrats who voted for Trump, while amongst Republicans, Clinton and Trump voters had more similar levels of cognitive reflection. Republicans who voted for third-party candidates or those who identified as libertarian had the highest levels of cognitive reflection.[40]

Physiology

People with right-wing views had greater skin conductance response, indicating greater sympathetic nervous system response, to threatening images than those with left-wing views in one study.[41] [1] There was no difference for positive or neutral images. Holding right-wing views was also associated with a stronger startle reflex as measured by strength of eyeblink in response to unexpected noise. Subsequent studies with substantially greater statistical power have failed to replicate these effects.[42] [43]

In an fMRI study published in Social Neuroscience, three different patterns of brain activation were found to correlate with individualism, conservatism, and radicalism.[44] In general, fMRI responses in several portions of the brain have been linked to viewing of the faces of well-known politicians.[45] Others believe that determining political affiliation from fMRI data is overreaching.[46]

Genetic studies

Heritability

Heritability compares differences in genetic factors in individuals to the total variance of observable characteristics ("phenotypes") in a population, to determine the heritability coefficient. Factors including genetics, environment and random chance can all contribute to the variation in individuals' phenotypes.[47]

The use of twin studies assumes the elimination of non-genetic differences by finding the statistical differences between monozygotic (identical) twins, which have almost the same genes, and dizygotic (fraternal) twins.[48] The similarity of the environment in which twins are reared has been questioned.[49] [50]

A twin study in 2005 by Alford et al. examined the attitudes regarding 28 different political issues such as capitalism, unions, X-rated movies, abortion, school prayer, divorce, property taxes, and the draft. Twins were asked if they agreed or disagreed or were uncertain about each issue. Genetic factors accounted for 53% of the variance of an overall score. However, self-identification as Republican and Democrat had a much lower heritability of 14%. It is worthwhile to note that identical twins correlated in opinion at a rate of 0.66 while fraternal twins correlated in opinion by 0.44. This likely occurs because identical twins share 100% of their DNA while fraternal twins share on average only 50% of their DNA.[51] [52] However, Jonathan Kaplan argued that the role of individual genes is often extremely small due to many human physical traits being polygenic and may be overstated,[53] observing that the study by Alford et al. made a case for the role of the 5-HTTLPR region being involved in numerous psychological and personality traits, yet Border et al. (2019) found that multiple associations with 5-HTTLPR were spurious and underpowered.[54]

Jost et al. wrote in a 2011 review that "Many studies involving quite diverse samples and methods suggest that political and religious views reflect a reasonably strong genetic basis, but this does not mean that ideological proclivities are unaffected by personal experiences or environmental factors."[1]

Gene association studies

In 2014, a study was performed on genomic data from 12,000 twin pairs from Australia, the USA, Denmark, Sweden and Hungary to examine genetic influences on political ideology. The study's genome-wide association analysis did not provide any definitive evidence of a specific genetic marker related to ideology. The authors remarked that, as with any complex trait, a single gene or small group of genes would not influence ideology directly but that there would likely be "thousands of genetic variants of very small effects and constellations of genes interacting with each other and the environment to influence behavior, indirectly".[55]

"A Genome-Wide Analysis of Liberal and Conservative Political Attitudes" by Peter K. Hatemi et al. traces DNA research involving 13,000 subjects. The study identifies several genes potentially connected with political ideology.[56]

Evolutionary psychology

From an evolutionary psychology perspective, conflicts regarding redistribution of wealth may have been a recurrent issue in the ancestral environment. Humans may therefore have developed psychological mechanisms for judging their chance of succeeding in such conflicts which will affect their political views. For males, physical strength may have been an important factor in deciding the outcome of such conflicts. Therefore, a prediction is that males that have high physical strength and low socioeconomic status (SES) will support redistribution while males that have both high SES and high physical strength will oppose redistribution. Cross-cultural research found this to be the case; for females, their physical strength had no influence on their political views which was as expected since females rarely have physical strength above that of the average male.[57] A study on political attitudes among Hollywood actors found that, while the actors were generally more left-leaning, male actors with great physical strength were more likely to support the Republican stance on foreign issues and foreign military interventions.[58]

An alternative evolutionary explanation for political diversity is that it is a polymorphism, like those of gender and blood type, resulting from frequency-dependent selection. Tim Dean has suggested that we live in such a moral ecosystem whereby the advantage gained by having any one particular moral strategy diminishes as it becomes very common, causing evolution to produce individuals with a diversity of moral strategies.[59] Alford et al. posit that political variation could offer groups different strategies of solving problems, thus variation is maintained by virtue of being adaptive at the group level.[60]

See also

Further reading

External links

Notes and References

  1. Jost. John T.. Amodio, David M.. Political ideology as motivated social cognition: Behavioral and neuroscientific evidence. Motivation and Emotion. 13 November 2011. 36. 1. 55–64. 10.1007/s11031-011-9260-7. 10675844.
  2. Web site: Dolan . Eric W. . 2024-04-10 . Genetic variations help explain the link between cognitive ability and liberalism . 2024-04-12 . PsyPost - Psychology News . en-US.
  3. Kleppesto . Thomas Haarklau . Czajkowski . Nikolai Olavi . Sheehy-Skeffington . Jennifer . Vassend . Olav . Roysamb . Espen . Eftedal . Nikolai Haahjem . Kunst . Jonas R. . Ystrom . Eivind . Thomsen . Lotte . 2024-02-22 . The genetic underpinnings of right-wing authoritarianism and social dominance orientation explain political attitudes beyond Big Five personality . Journal of Personality . en . 10.1111/jopy.12921 . 38386613 . 0022-3506. 10852/111202 . free .
  4. Biology and ideology: The anatomy of politics . Buchen . Lizzie . 2012 . Nature . 490 . 7421 . 466–468 . en . 10.1038/490466a . 23099382. 2012Natur.490..466B . free .
  5. Davies . James . 1983 . The Proper Biological Study of Politics . Political Psychology . 4 . 4 . 731–743 . 10.2307/3791065 . 3791065 .
  6. Political Orientations Are Correlated with Brain Structure in Young Adults . Kanai. R. Feilden. T. Firth. C. Rees. G. Current Biology. 2011 . 10.1016/j.cub.2011.03.017 . 21 . 8 . 677 – 80 . 21474316 . 3092984 . 2011CBio...21..677K .
  7. Ninh, Amie (April 8, 2011). "Liberal vs. Conservative: Does the Difference Lie in the Brain?". Time.
  8. Book: Carlson, Neil R.. Physiology of Behavior. 12 January 2012. Pearson. 978-0205239399. 364.
  9. Bzdok D, Langner R, Caspers S, Kurth F, Habel U, Zilles K, Laird A, Eickhoff SB . ALE meta-analysis on facial judgments of trustworthiness and attractiveness . Brain Structure & Function . 215 . 3–4 . 209–23 . January 2011 . 20978908 . 4020344 . 10.1007/s00429-010-0287-4 .
  10. Kennedy DP, Gläscher J, Tyszka JM, Adolphs R . Personal space regulation by the human amygdala . Nature Neuroscience . 12 . 10 . 1226–7 . October 2009 . 19718035 . 2753689 . 10.1038/nn.2381 .
  11. Bickart KC, Wright CI, Dautoff RJ, Dickerson BC, Barrett LF . Amygdala volume and social network size in humans . Nature Neuroscience . 14 . 2 . 163–4 . February 2011 . 21186358 . 3079404 . 10.1038/nn.2724 .
  12. How to Win Friends: Have a Big Amygdala?. Time. Maia. Szalavitz. 28 December 2010. 30 December 2010. live. https://web.archive.org/web/20110717061203/http://healthland.time.com/2010/12/28/how-to-win-friends-have-a-big-amygdala/?xid=rss-topstories. 17 July 2011. dmy-all.
  13. T.L. Brink. (2008) Psychology: A Student Friendly Approach. "Unit 4: The Nervous System". pp 61.
  14. Feinstein JS, Adolphs R, Damasio A, Tranel D . The human amygdala and the induction and experience of fear . Current Biology . 21 . 1 . 34–8 . January 2011 . 21167712 . 3030206 . 10.1016/j.cub.2010.11.042 . 2011CBio...21...34F .
  15. Staut CC, Naidich TP . 46862405 . Urbach-Wiethe disease (Lipoid proteinosis) . Pediatric Neurosurgery . 28 . 4 . 212–4 . April 1998 . 9732251 . 10.1159/000028653 .
  16. Conservatives are more easily disgusted than liberals. . Y. Inbar . Cognition and Emotion . 2008. 23. 4 . 714–725 . 10.1080/02699930802110007. etal. 10.1.1.372.3053 . 7411404 .
  17. Sanfey AG, Rilling JK, Aronson JA, Nystrom LE, Cohen JD . 7111382 . The neural basis of economic decision-making in the Ultimatum Game . Science . 300 . 5626 . 1755–8 . June 2003 . 12805551 . 10.1126/science.1082976 . 2003Sci...300.1755S .
  18. Both of us disgusted in My insula: The common neural basis of seeing and feeling disgust. B. Wicker. Neuron. 2003. 40. 3. 655–664. etal. 10.1016/s0896-6273(03)00679-2. 14642287. free.
  19. Wright P, He G, Shapira NA, Goodman WK, Liu Y . Disgust and the insula: fMRI responses to pictures of mutilation and contamination . NeuroReport . 15 . 15 . 2347–51 . October 2004 . 15640753 . 10.1097/00001756-200410250-00009. 6864309 .
  20. Lane RD, Reiman EM, Axelrod B, Yun LS, Holmes A, Schwartz GE . Neural correlates of levels of emotional awareness. Evidence of an interaction between emotion and attention in the anterior cingulate cortex . Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience . 10 . 4 . 525–35 . July 1998 . 9712681 . 10.1162/089892998562924 . 27743177 .
  21. Price DD . 15250446 . Psychological and neural mechanisms of the affective dimension of pain . Science . 288 . 5472 . 1769–72 . June 2000 . 10846154 . 10.1126/science.288.5472.1769 . 2000Sci...288.1769P .
  22. Neural activity in the human brain relating to uncertainty and arousal during anticipation. . H. Critchley . Neuron . 2001. 29. 2 . 537–545 . 10.1016/s0896-6273(01)00225-2. 11239442 . etal. 21.11116/0000-0001-A313-1 . 10995076 . free .
  23. Bush G, Luu P, Posner MI . Cognitive and emotional influences in anterior cingulate cortex . Trends in Cognitive Sciences . 4 . 6 . 215–222 . June 2000 . 10827444 . 10.1016/S1364-6613(00)01483-2 . 16451230 .
  24. Web site: Politics on the Brain: Scans Show Whether You Lean Left or Right . 7 April 2011 . . September 25, 2012.
  25. News: The liberal brain? Scans show liberals and conservatives have different brain structures . . April 8, 2011 . September 25, 2012 . Kattalia, Kathryn.
  26. Why Are Conservatives Happier Than Liberals? . Napier J . Jost JT . Psychological Science. 2008. 19. 6. 562-72 . 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2008.02124.x .
  27. Political leanings vary with facial expression processing and psychosocial functioning. . J. Vigil . Group Processes & Intergroup Relations . 2010. 13. 5 . 547–558 . 10.1177/1368430209356930. 59571553 . etal.
  28. The end of the end of ideology. . J. Jost . American Psychologist. 2006. 61. 7. 651–670 . 10.1037/0003-066x.61.7.651. etal . 17032067.
  29. Political conservatism as motivated social cognition. . J. Jost . Psychological Bulletin . 2003. 129. 3. 339–375 . 10.1037/0033-2909.129.3.339 . 12784934. 1778256 . etal.
  30. David M Amodio, John T Jost, Sarah L Master & Cindy M Yee, Neurocognitive correlates of liberalism and conservatism, Nature Neuroscience. Cited by 69 other studies
  31. Web site: Brains of Liberals, Conservatives May Work Differently. https://web.archive.org/web/20161013125516/http://psychcentral.com/news/2007/09/10/brains-of-liberals-conservatives-may-work-differently/1691.html. 2016-10-13. Psych Central. 2007-10-20.
  32. News: Study finds left-wing brain, right-wing brain. Los Angeles Times. 2007-09-10.
  33. Bernabel, R. T., & Oliveira, A. (2017). Conservatism and liberalism predict performance in two nonideological cognitive tasks. Politics and the Life Sciences, 36(2), 49-59.
  34. Weissflog, M., Choma, B. L., Dywan, J., van Noordt, S. J., & Segalowitz, S. J. (2013). The political (and physiological) divide: Political orientation, performance monitoring, and the anterior cingulate response. Social neuroscience, 8(5), 434-447.
  35. Inzlicht, Michael, Ian McGregor, Jacob B. Hirsh, and Kyle Nash. "Neural markers of religious conviction." Psychological science 20, no. 3 (2009): 385-392.
  36. Kremláček, Jan, Daniel Musil, Jana Langrová, and Martin Palecek. "Neural correlates of liberalism and conservatism in a post-Communist country." Frontiers in Human Neuroscience 13 (2019): 119.
  37. Wendell, D. G. (2016). Loyola eCommons Neural Correlates of Political Attitudes: Emotion and Ideology in the Brain. Available online at: http://ecommons.luc.edu/luc_diss/2299, Loyola University Chicago, pages 133-178 Accessed 27/07/2023
  38. Xu, Xiaowen, Annika K. Karinen, Hanah A. Chapman, Jordan B. Peterson, and Jason E. Plaks. "An orderly personality partially explains the link between trait disgust and political conservatism." Cognition and Emotion (2019).
  39. Elad-Strenger, Julia, Jutta Proch, and Thomas Kessler. "Is Disgust a "Conservative" Emotion?." Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin (2019): 0146167219880191.
  40. [Gordon Pennycook|Pennycook, Gordon]
  41. Oxley . D. R. . Smith . K. B. . Alford . J. R. . Hibbing . M. V. . Miller . J. L. . Scalora . M. . Hatemi . P. K. . Hibbing . J. R. . Political Attitudes Vary with Physiological Traits . Science . 19 September 2008 . 321 . 5896 . 1667–1670 . 10.1126/science.1157627. 18801995 . 2008Sci...321.1667O . 7215269 .
  42. Bakker . Bert N. . Schumacher . Gijs . Gothreau . Claire . Arceneaux . Kevin . Conservatives and liberals have similar physiological responses to threats . Nature Human Behaviour . June 2020 . 4 . 6 . 613–621 . 10.1038/s41562-020-0823-z. 32042109 . 7306406 .
  43. Fournier . Patrick . Soroka . Stuart . Nir . Lilach . Negativity Biases and Political Ideology: A Comparative Test across 17 Countries . American Political Science Review . August 2020 . 114 . 3 . 775–791 . 10.1017/S0003055420000131. 225441710 .
  44. Individualism, conservatism, and radicalism as criteria for processing political beliefs: a parametric fMRI study . 19562629 . 10.1080/17470910902860308 . 4 . 5 . 2009 . 367–83 . Zamboni G, Gozzi M, Krueger F, Duhamel JR, Sirigu A, Grafman J . Social Neuroscience. 15652205 . Zamboni G, Gozzi M, Krueger F, Duhamel JR, Sirigu A, Jordan Grafman. National Institutes of Health, Bethesda, MD, USA
  45. 1828689. Politics on the Brain: An fMRI Investigation. Kristine Knudson. March 2006. 17372621. 10.1080/17470910600670603. 1. 1. Soc Neurosci. 25–40. etal.
  46. 19232374 . 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2008.12.017 . 73 . 1 . Great expectations: what can fMRI research tell us about psychological phenomena? . July 2009 . 10–6 . Aue T, Lavelle LA, Cacioppo JT . International Journal of Psychophysiology.
  47. Raj . A . van Oudenaarden . A . 2008 . Nature, nurture, or chance: stochastic gene expression and its consequences . Cell. 135. 2. 216–26. 10.1016/j.cell.2008.09.050 . 18957198 . 3118044.
  48. Martin . Nicholas . Boomsma . Dorret . Machin . Geoffrey . A twin-pronged attack on complex traits . . 17 December 1997 . 17 . 4 . 387–92 . 10.1038/ng1297-387 . 9398838 . 2028886 .
  49. Beckwith . Jon . Morris . Corey A. . Twin Studies of Political Behavior: Untenable Assumptions? . Perspectives on Politics . December 2008 . 6 . 4 . 785–91 . 10.1017/S1537592708081917 . 55630117 .
  50. Book: Fiske . Susan T. . Gilbert . Daniel T. . Lindzey . Gardner . Handbook of Social Psychology . 15 February 2010 . John Wiley & Sons . 372 . 5th .
  51. News: Some Politics May Be Etched in the Genes . . June 21, 2005 . September 25, 2012 . Carey, Benedict.
  52. Alford . J. R. . Funk . C. L. . Hibbing . J. R. . Are Political Orientations Genetically Transmitted? . 10.1017/S0003055405051579 . American Political Science Review . 99 . 2 . 153–167 . 2005 . 10.1.1.622.476 . 3820911 .
  53. Kincaid, Harold, and Jeroen Van Bouwel, eds. The Oxford Handbook of Philosophy of Political Science. Oxford University Press, 2023, pg 19-32
  54. Border, Richard, Emma C. Johnson, Luke M. Evans, Andrew Smolen, Noah Berley, Patrick F. Sullivan, and Matthew C. Keller. "No support for historical candidate gene or candidate gene-by-interaction hypotheses for major depression across multiple large samples." American Journal of Psychiatry 176, no. 5 (2019): 376-387.
  55. Genetic Influences on Political Ideologies: Twin Analyses of 19 Measures of Political Ideologies from Five Democracies and Genome-Wide Findings from Three Populations . Hatemi. PK. Medland. SE. Klemmensen. R. Oskarrson. S. Littvay. L. Dawes. C. Verhulst. B. McDermott. R. Nørgaard. AS. Klofstad. C. Christensen. K. Johannesson. M. Magnusson. PKE. Eaves. LJ. Martin. NG . Behav Genet . 2014 . 44 . 3 . 282–294 . 24569950 . 10.1007/s10519-014-9648-8 . 4038932 .
  56. Hatemi . P. K. . Gillespie . N. A. . Eaves . L. J. . Maher . B. S. . Webb . B. T. . Heath . A. C. . Medland . S. E. . Smyth . D. C. . Beeby . H. N. . Gordon . S. D. . Montgomery . G. W. . Zhu . G. . Byrne . E. M. . Martin . N. G. . A Genome-Wide Analysis of Liberal and Conservative Political Attitudes . 10.1017/S0022381610001015 . The Journal of Politics . 73 . 271–285 . 2011 . 10.1.1.662.2987 .
  57. Michael Bang Petersen. The evolutionary psychology of Mass Politics. In Book: Roberts . S. C. . Roberts . 10.1093/acprof:oso/9780199586073.001.0001 . S. Craig . Applied Evolutionary Psychology . 2011 . Oxford University Press. 9780199586073 .
  58. Web site: Strong men more likely to vote Conservative . . April 11, 2012 . September 25, 2012.
  59. Dean . T. . Evolution and Moral Diversity . 10.4148/biyclc.v7i0.1775 . The Baltic International Yearbook of Cognition, Logic and Communication . 7 . 2012 . free .
  60. Alford, John R., Carolyn L. Funk, and John R. Hibbing. "Are political orientations genetically transmitted?." American political science review 99, no. 2 (2005): 153-167.