Approximant Explained

pronounced as /notice/

Approximants are speech sounds that involve the articulators approaching each other but not narrowly enough nor with enough articulatory precision[1] to create turbulent airflow. Therefore, approximants fall between fricatives, which do produce a turbulent airstream, and vowels, which produce no turbulence. This class is composed of sounds like pronounced as /[ɹ]/ (as in rest) and semivowels like pronounced as /[j]/ and pronounced as /[w]/ (as in yes and west, respectively), as well as lateral approximants like pronounced as /[l]/ (as in less).

Terminology

Before Peter Ladefoged coined the term approximant in the 1960s,[2] the terms frictionless continuant and semivowel were used to refer to non-lateral approximants.

In phonology, approximant is also a distinctive feature that encompasses all sonorants except nasals, including vowels, taps, and trills.

Semivowels

See main article: Semivowel.

Some approximants resemble vowels in acoustic and articulatory properties and the terms semivowel and glide are often used for these non-syllabic vowel-like segments. The correlation between semivowels and vowels is strong enough that cross-language differences between semivowels correspond with the differences between their related vowels.[3]

Vowels and their corresponding semivowels alternate in many languages depending on the phonological environment, or for grammatical reasons, as is the case with Indo-European ablaut. Similarly, languages often avoid configurations where a semivowel precedes its corresponding vowel.[4] A number of phoneticians distinguish between semivowels and approximants by their location in a syllable. Although he uses the terms interchangeably, remarks that, for example, the final glides of English par and buy differ from French par ('through') and baille ('tub') in that, in the latter pair, the approximants appear in the syllable coda, whereas, in the former, they appear in the syllable nucleus. This means that opaque (if not minimal) contrasts can occur in languages like Italian (with the i-like sound of piede 'foot', appearing in the nucleus: pronounced as /[ˈpi̯ɛˑde]/, and that of piano 'plan', appearing in the syllable onset: pronounced as /[ˈpjaˑno]/) and Spanish (with a near minimal pair being abyecto pronounced as /[aβˈjekto]/ 'abject' and abierto pronounced as /[aˈβi̯erto]/ 'opened').

+Approximant-vowel correspondencesVowelCorresponding
approximant
Place of
articulation
Example
pronounced as /link/ pronounced as /link/Spanish amplío ('I extend') vs. ampl ('he extended')
pronounced as /link/ pronounced as /link/ French aigu ('sharp') vs. aiguille ('needle')
pronounced as /link/ pronounced as /link/Korean 식 ('food') vs. 사 ('doctor')
pronounced as /link/ pronounced as /link/ Spanish continúo ('I continue') vs. continuó ('he/she/it continued') and ('you continued') used only in the formal treatment of 'usted'.
pronounced as /link/ pronounced as /link/
pronounced as /link/ pronounced as /link/ North American English waiter vs. waitress

Because of the articulatory complexities of the American English rhotic, there is some variation in its phonetic description. A transcription with the IPA character for an alveolar approximant (pronounced as /[ɹ]/) is common, though the sound is more postalveolar. Actual retroflexion may occur as well and both occur as variations of the same sound.[5] However, makes a distinction between the vowels of American English (which he calls "rhotacized") and vowels with "retroflexion" such as those that appear in Badaga;, on the other hand, labels both as r-colored and notes that both have a lowered third formant.[6]

Because the vowels pronounced as /[i ɯ]/ are articulated with spread lips, spreading is implied for their approximant analogues, pronounced as /[j ɰ]/. However, these sounds generally have little or no lip-spreading. The fricative letters with a lowering diacritic, (IPA|ʝ˕ ɣ˕), may therefore be justified for a neutral articulation between spread pronounced as /[j ɰ]/ and rounded pronounced as /[ɥ w]/.[7]

In articulation and often diachronically, palatal approximants correspond to front vowels, velar approximants to back vowels, and labialized approximants to rounded vowels. In American English, the rhotic approximant corresponds to the rhotic vowel. This can create alternations (as shown in the above table).

In addition to alternations, glides can be inserted to the left or the right of their corresponding vowels when they occur next to a hiatus. For example, in Ukrainian, medial pronounced as //i// triggers the formation of an inserted pronounced as /[j]/ that acts as a syllable onset so that when the affix pronounced as //-ist// is added to футбол ('football') to make футболіст 'football player', it is pronounced pronounced as /[futbo̞ˈlist]/, but маоїст ('Maoist'), with the same affix, is pronounced pronounced as /[mao̞ˈ'''j'''ist]/ with a glide. Dutch for many speakers has a similar process that extends to mid vowels:

Similarly, vowels can be inserted next to their corresponding glide in certain phonetic environments. Sievers' law describes this behaviour for Germanic.

Non-high semivowels also occur. In colloquial Nepali speech, a process of glide-formation occurs, where one of two adjacent vowels becomes non-syllabic; the process includes mid vowels so that pronounced as /[dʱo̯a]/ ('cause to wish') features a non-syllabic mid vowel. Spanish features a similar process and even nonsyllabic pronounced as //a// can occur so that ahorita ('right away') is pronounced pronounced as /[a̯o̞ˈɾita]/. It is not often clear, however, whether such sequences involve a semivowel (a consonant) or a diphthong (a vowel), and in many cases, it may not be a meaningful distinction.

Although many languages have central vowels pronounced as /[ɨ, ʉ]/, which lie between back/velar pronounced as /[ɯ, u]/ and front/palatal pronounced as /[i, y]/, there are few cases of a corresponding approximant pronounced as /[ȷ̈]/. One is in the Korean diphthong pronounced as /[ȷ̈i]/ or pronounced as /[ɨ̯i]/[9] though it is more frequently analyzed as velar (as in the table above), and Mapudungun may be another, with three high vowel sounds, pronounced as //i//, pronounced as //u//, pronounced as //ɨ// and three corresponding consonants, pronounced as //j//, and pronounced as //w//, and a third one is often described as a non-labialized voiced velar fricative; some texts note a correspondence between this approximant and pronounced as //ɨ// that is parallel to pronounced as //j//–pronounced as //i// and pronounced as //w//–pronounced as //u//. An example is liq pronounced as //ˈliɣ// (pronounced as /[ˈliɨ̯]/?) ('white').[10] It has been noted that the expected symbols for the approximant correlates of pronounced as /[ɨ], [ʉ]/ are (IPA|ɉ, ɥ̶)[11] or (IPA|ɉ, w̶).[12]

Approximants versus fricatives

In addition to less turbulence, approximants also differ from fricatives in the precision required to produce them. When emphasized, approximants may be slightly fricated (that is, the airstream may become slightly turbulent), which is reminiscent of fricatives. For example, the Spanish word ayuda ('help') features a palatal approximant that is pronounced as a fricative in emphatic speech. Spanish can be analyzed as having a meaningful distinction between fricative, approximant, and intermediate pronounced as //ʝ ʝ˕ j//.[13] However, such frication is generally slight and intermittent, unlike the strong turbulence of fricative consonants.

For places of articulation further back in the mouth, languages do not contrast voiced fricatives and approximants. Therefore, the IPA allows the symbols for the voiced fricatives to double for the approximants, with or without a lowering diacritic.

Occasionally, the glottal "fricatives" are called approximants, since pronounced as /[h]/ typically has no more frication than voiceless approximants, but they are often phonations of the glottis without any accompanying manner or place of articulation.

Central approximants

Approximants with a dedicated IPA symbol are in bold.

Lateral approximants

In lateral approximants, the center of tongue makes solid contact with the roof of the mouth. However, the defining location is the side of the tongue, which only approaches the teeth, allowing free passage of air.

Coarticulated approximants

Voiceless approximants

Voiceless approximants are not recognized by all phoneticians as a discrete phonetic category. There are problems in distinguishing voiceless approximants from voiceless fricatives.

Phonetic characteristics

Fricative consonants are generally said to be the result of turbulent airflow at a place of articulation in the vocal tract. However, an audible voiceless sound may be made without this turbulent airflow: makes a distinction between "local friction" (as in pronounced as /[s]/ or pronounced as /[z]/) and "cavity friction" (as in voiceless vowels like pronounced as /[ḁ]/ and pronounced as /[ɔ̥]/). More recent research distinguishes between "turbulent" and "laminar" airflow in the vocal tract. It is not clear if it is possible to describe voiceless approximants categorically as having laminar airflow (or cavity friction in Pike's terms) as a way of distinguishing them from fricatives. write that "the airflow for voiced approximants remains laminar (smooth), and does not become turbulent. Voiceless approximants are rare in the languages of the world, but when they do occur the airflow is usually somewhat turbulent." Audible voiceless sounds may also be produced by means of turbulent airflow at the glottis, as in pronounced as /[h]/; in such a case, it is possible to articulate an audible voiceless sound without the production of local friction at a supraglottal constriction. describes such sounds, but classes them as sonorants.

Distinctiveness

Voiceless approximants are rarely if ever distinguished phonemically from voiceless fricatives in the sound system of a language. discuss the issue and conclude "In practice, it is difficult to distinguish between a voiceless approximant and a voiceless fricative at the same place of articulation ... there is no evidence that any language in the world makes such a distinction crucial."

Disagreement over use of the term

Voiceless approximants are treated as a phonetic category by (among others),, and . However, the term voiceless approximant is seen by some phoneticians as controversial. It has been pointed out that if approximant is defined as a speech sound that involves the articulators approaching each other but not narrowly enough to create turbulent airflow, then it is difficult to see how a voiceless approximant could be audible. As John C. Wells puts it in his blog, "voiceless approximants are by definition inaudible ... If there's no friction and no voicing, there's nothing to hear."[15] A similar point is made in relation to frictionless continuants by : "There are no voiceless frictionless continuants because this would imply silence; the voiceless counterpart of the frictionless continuant is the voiceless fricative." argue that the increased airflow arising from voicelessness alone makes a voiceless continuant a fricative, even if lacking a greater constriction in the oral cavity than a voiced approximant.

argue that Burmese and Standard Tibetan have voiceless lateral approximants pronounced as /[l̥]/ and Navajo and Zulu voiceless lateral fricatives pronounced as /link/, but also say that "in other cases it is difficult to decide whether a voiceless lateral should be described as an approximant or a fricative". compared voiceless laterals in Estonian Swedish, Icelandic, and Welsh and found that Welsh-speakers consistently used pronounced as /[ɬ]/, that Icelandic-speakers consistently used pronounced as /[l̥]/, and that speakers of Estonian Swedish varied in their pronunciation. They conclude that there is "a range of variants within voiceless laterals, rather than a categorical split between lateral fricatives and voiceless approximant laterals".

Occurrence in Western American English

Voiceless lateral approximants can occur after voiceless stops as allophone of its voiced counterpart, especially after the voiceless velar plosive pronounced as //k//, in Western American English.

Nasalized approximants

Examples are:

In Portuguese, the nasal glides pronounced as /[j̃]/ and pronounced as /[w̃]/ historically became pronounced as //ɲ// and pronounced as //m// in some words. In Edo, the nasalized allophones of the approximants pronounced as //j// and pronounced as //w// are nasal occlusives, pronounced as /[ɲ]/ and pronounced as /[ŋʷ]/.

What are transcribed as nasal approximants may include non-syllabic elements of nasal vowels or diphthongs.

See also

References

pronounced as /navigation/

Notes and References

  1. , citing
  2. , pointing to
  3. , citing
  4. , citing
  5. citing,, and
  6. Both cited in
  7. John Esling (2010) "Phonetic Notation", in Hardcastle, Laver & Gibbon (eds) The Handbook of Phonetic Sciences, 2nd ed., p. 699.
  8. There is dialectal and allophonic variation in the realization of pronounced as //ʋ//. For speakers who realize it as pronounced as /[ʋ]/, postulates an additional rule that changes any occurrence of pronounced as /[w]/ from glide insertion into pronounced as /[ʋ]/.
  9. Web site: Ahn & Iverson (2006) . 2010-12-31 . https://web.archive.org/web/20110720114729/https://pantherfile.uwm.edu/iverson/www/kordiph.pdf . 2011-07-20 .
  10. Listen to a recording
  11. Martin Ball & Joan Rahilly (2011) The symbolization of central approximants in the IPA. Journal of the International Phonetic Association. 41 (2), p. 231 - 237.
  12. Book: Ball . Martin J. . Phonetics for communication disorders . Müller . Nicole . Psychology Press . 2011 . 978-0-8058-5363-6 . 70.
  13. Martínez-Celdrán, E. (2004) "Problems in the classification of approximants". Journal of the International Phonetic Association, 34, 201–10.
  14. There have been repeated requests that the IPA created dedicated symbols for pronounced as /[β̞]/ and pronounced as /[ð̞]/ – typically modifications of the base letters such as turned pronounced as /⟨/pronounced as /β/pronounced as /⟩/ and pronounced as /⟨/pronounced as /ð/pronounced as /⟩/ or reversed (IPA|β) and (IPA|ð) – but so far the IPA has deemed that there is insufficient need for them.
  15. Web site: Wells . J. C. . [h]

    Fricative or approximant? ]

    . John Wells' Blog . 7 April 2009 . 23 December 2020.