Warrongo | |
Also Known As: | Northern Maric |
States: | Australia |
Region: | Queensland, west of Ingham and Abergowrie almost to Einasleigh |
Ethnicity: | Warrongo, Gugu-Badhun, Gudjal |
Extinct: | 1981, with the death of Alf Palmer |
Familycolor: | Australian |
Fam1: | Pama–Nyungan |
Fam2: | Maric |
Dia1: | Warungu |
Dia2: | Gugu-Badhun |
Dia3: | Gudjal (Gudjala) |
Lc1: | wrg |
Ld1: | Warungu |
Lc2: | gdc |
Ld2: | Gugu-Badhun |
Aiatsis: | Y133 |
Aiatsisname: | Warungu |
Aiatsis2: | Y128 |
Aiatsisname2: | Gugu Badhun |
Glotto: | nort2757 |
Glottorefname: | Northern Maric |
Warrongo (or War(r)ungu) is an Australian Aboriginal language, one of the dozen languages of the Maric branch of the Pama–Nyungan family.[1] It was formerly spoken by the Warrongo people in the area around Townsville, Queensland, Australia. Its last native speaker was Alf Palmer, who died in 1981.
Before his death, linguists Tasaku Tsunoda and Peter Sutton worked together with Palmer to preserve the language (Warrungu proper); thanks to their efforts, the language is beginning to be revived.
One of the notable feature of the language is its syntactic ergativity.
As noted by Ethnologue, the language is currently dormant meaning that there are no native/proficient speakers left.
Alternative names for the language include Warrangu, Warrango, War(r)uŋu, War-oong-oo, Gudjala and Gudjal.
The Warungu language region includes areas from the Upper Herbert River to Mount Garnet.
Nowadays people identifying themselves as Warrongo live both within traditional Warrongo territory (Mount Garnet) and outside it (Palm Island, Townsville, Ingham, Cardwell, and Cairns). The language has been extinct since the last speaker, Alf Palmer, died in 1981. In the late 1990s or early 2000s a language revival movement started by a community of people, most of them grandchildren of the last speakers, the central figure being Rachel Cummins, the granddaughter of Alf Palmer.[2] The community had contacted Tsunoda, the linguist who worked with the last speakers in the 1970s, and between 2002 and 2006 he conducted 5 sessions of lessons, of 4–5 days each. As a result, the language seems to have acquired a limited set of symbolic functions. It has begun to be used in teasing between children, and as a source of personal names.
There appear to have been at least two mutually intelligible dialects. Warrongo belongs to the Pama-Nyungan (macro)family. The most closely related languages are Gugu Badhun (90% lexical sharing in terms of Hale's 99-item vocabulary) and Gujal (94% lexical sharing). The intermediate level classification of this group seems uncertain: the evidence from phonological correspondences, pronouns and verb roots suggests it belongs to the Maric group (alongside Bidjara, Gungabula, Marganj, Gunja, Biri and Nyaygungu), while the verbal inflectional morphology is akin to that of the Hebert River group (which includes Dyirbal, Warrgamay, Nyawaygi and Manbarra). It has been suggested that the verbal inflectional suffixes might have been the result of massive borrowing.
bilabial | lamino-dental | apico-alveolar | retroflex | lamino-palatal | dorso-velar | ||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
stop | pronounced as /link/ | (pronounced as /link/) | pronounced as /link/ | pronounced as /link/ | pronounced as /link/ | ||
nasal | pronounced as /link/ | (pronounced as /link/) | pronounced as /link/ | pronounced as /link/ | pronounced as /link/ | ||
rhotic | pronounced as /link/ | pronounced as /link/ | |||||
lateral | pronounced as /link/ | ||||||
semivowel | pronounced as /link/ | pronounced as /link/ |
The sound pronounced as /[h]/ appears only in the interjection pronounced as /[hai]/ 'Hi!' and the exclamation of surprise pronounced as /[haha]/ (or pronounced as /[ha:ha:]/) Dentalized consonants tend to appear in the Gugu-Badhun dialect. An alveolar approximant pronounced as /[ɹ]/ is stated to appear in the Gugu-Badhun dialect as well. The retroflex approximant pronounced as //ɻ// in syllable-final position can infrequently be realised as a retroflex tap pronounced as /[ɽ]/. The lamino-palatal stop is in most instances phonetically an affricate pronounced as /[tʃ]/ or pronounced as /[dʒ]/.Voicing is not distinctive for stops . The rules for voicing are fairly complex, but still it is impossible to predict it in all instances . The factors involved are the place of articulation (the more front the stop, the more likely it is to be voiced), the phonetic environment, position with respect to word boundaries, and possibly also the length of the word, the number of syllables that follow the stop and the location of stress.
There are three vowels: pronounced as //a//, pronounced as //i// and pronounced as //u// (orthographically). Length is distinctive only for pronounced as //a//, its long counterpart is orthographically represented as . pronounced as //u// has two allophones: pronounced as /[u]/, and pronounced as /[o]/ (neither of which involve significant lip rounding), depending on the preceding consonant. Both are possible after pronounced as //b//, pronounced as //m// and pronounced as //j//, while after all other consonants only pronounced as /[o]/ appears. The allophony of pronounced as //i// seems to be governed by more complex rules but generally, pronounced as /[i]/ is the sole allophone after pronounced as //ɟ//ˌ pronounced as //ɲ//ˌ pronounced as //ŋ// and pronounced as //w//, while after almost all other consonants both pronounced as /[i]/ and pronounced as /[e]/ can be observed.
Warrongo is analysed as having five word classes: nouns, (personal) pronouns, adverbs, verbs and interjections. Most of these contain interrogative and demonstrative members; example of an interrogative noun is ngani 'what', ngoni 'there' is a demonstrative adverb, an interrogative verb is ngani-nga-L 'to do what', and a demonstrative one is yama-nga-L 'to do thus'. Almost all words belong exclusively to a word class, while change of word class is achieved through derivational suffixes. Adjectives do not form a separate class as they share the morphology and syntactic behaviour of nouns. There are also about a dozen enclitics, with a range of functions: emphasis, focus, intensification, or meanings like 'only', 'enough', 'too', 'I don't know', 'counterfactual'.
Nouns generally do not distinguish number or gender, while pronouns have different forms for number (singular, dual and plural) and person (first, second and third). All of them do, however, inflect for case. The case suffixes have allomorphs according to the final phoneme of the stem, with some peculiarities exhibited by pronouns and by vowel-final proper and kin nouns . There are also a few irregular nouns.
Nouns have a single form, unmarked by a suffix, for the nominative case (used for the subject of an intransitive verb) and the accusative case (used for the object of a transitive verb), while the ergative case (used for the subject of a transitive verb) is marked by a suffix. In pronouns, on the other hand, the nominative and the ergative coincide in the bare stem form, while the accusative is marked by a suffix. Exceptionally, the third person dual and plural pronouns, as well as vowel-final proper and kin nouns, receive separate marking for each of these three cases. The ergative, if used with inanimate nouns, may also mark an instrument.
The locative case describes path or destination of movement, location, duration in time, instrument (and means), company ('together with'), and cause or reason. The dative case marks purpose, cause and reason, possession (rarely), goal and direction of movement, recipient, temporal duration or endpoint, a core argument in some syntactic constructions, and a complement of intransitives verbs or nouns like 'fond (of)', 'good (to)', 'know', 'forget'. The genitive is used only with animate nouns and marks, besides the typical possessor and related functions, also a beneficiary, recipient, or complement of some verbs and nouns. The ablative most commonly marks reason or a temporal or spatial starting point. The comitative seems to have a wide range of meanings, some of them idiomatic, but the most typical seem to correspond to English 'with'.
Genitive, ablative and comitative suffixes may be followed by other case suffixes.Some adverbs can take case suffixes: locative (optionally for adverbs of place), dative (with the sense 'to', optionally for adverbs of place, obligatory for adverbs of time), or ablative (obligatory for both if the meaning is 'from, since'). Adverbs of manner cannot take case suffixes – this distinguishes them from nouns that express similar meanings (as these nouns must agree in case with the nouns they modify).
Verbs belong to one of three conjugation classes, which are characterised by the presence of a 'conjugational marker' (-l-, -y- or none) which appears in certain verb forms. Verbs take suffixes for change of valency or for tense/mood (future tense, between two and three non-future tenses, imperatives, apprehensional). There are also purposive forms, which signal intention when used as the predicate of a non-subordinate clause, or mark verbs in subordinate clauses for purpose, result or successive actions.
Word order is free and does not seem to be governed by information structure. Constituents of a single phrase need not be contiguous. There are however some tendencies. Numeral nouns usually follow the head noun, while adjective-like modifiers tend to precede it. Arguments tend to precede verbs, while the agent-like argument of a transitive verb more often than not precedes the patient-like argument, although more frequently only one of them is expressed.
The three most common means of joining clauses are sentence-sequence (juxtaposed clauses that have separate intonation contours), coordination (juxtaposed clauses with one intonation contour and sharing of conjugational categories such as tense) and subordination. The most common type of subordination is the purposive.
If there are shared arguments, they are more likely to be deleted from the second clause if it is subordinate, and least likely if it is sentence-sequence. The restrictions on the syntactic function of the shared argument are typical of syntactically ergative languages. The shared argument has to have the same function in both clauses, or be an intransitive subject (S) in one and a transitive patient-like argument (O) in the other:
In case the shared argument is a transitive agent-like argument (A) in one of the clauses, antipassivisation will be involved. It is signalled by a verbal suffix and affects the case marking of the arguments of this verb. In comparison with the basic verb, which marks the A with ergative/nominative and the O with nominative/accusative, the antipassivised verb marks the A with nominative and the O with either ergative or dative. The agent-like argument then becomes available to be coreferential with a patient of a transitive verb or a subject of an intransitive one: