pronounced as /notice/thumb|right|300px|Spanish dialects in Colombia.thumb|right|300px|Spanish dialects spoken in Venezuela.Some of the regional varieties of the Spanish language are quite divergent from one another, especially in pronunciation and vocabulary, and less so in grammar.
While all Spanish dialects adhere to approximately the same written standard, all spoken varieties differ from the written variety, to different degrees. There are differences between European Spanish (also called Peninsular Spanish) and the Spanish of the Americas, as well as many different dialect areas both within Spain and within the Americas. Chilean and Honduran Spanish have been identified by various linguists as the most divergent varieties.[1]
Prominent differences in pronunciation among dialects of Spanish include:
Among grammatical features, the most prominent variation among dialects is in the use of the second-person pronouns. In Hispanic America the only second-person plural pronoun, for both formal and informal treatment, is, while in most of Spain the informal second-person plural pronoun is with used only in the formal treatment. For the second-person singular familiar pronoun, some American dialects use (and its associated verb forms), while others use either (see voseo) or both tú and (which, together with usted, can make for a possible three-tiered distinction of formalities).
There are significant differences in vocabulary among regional varieties of Spanish, particularly in the domains of food products, everyday objects, and clothes; and many American varieties show considerable lexical influence from Native American languages.
While there is no broad consensus on how Latin American Spanish dialects should be classified, the following scheme which takes into account phonological, grammatical, socio-historical, and language contact data provides a reasonable approximation of Latin American dialect variation:
While there are other types of regional variation in Peninsular Spanish, and the Spanish of bilingual regions shows influence from other languages, the greatest division in Old World varieties is from north to south, with a central-northern dialect north of Madrid, an Andalusian dialect to the south, and an intermediary region between the two most important dialect zones. Meanwhile, the Canary Islands constitute their own dialect cluster, whose speech is most closely related to that of western Andalusia.[2]
The non-native Spanish in Equatorial Guinea and Western Sahara (formerly Spanish Sahara) has been influenced mainly by varieties from Spain. Spanish is also an official language in Equatorial Guinea, and many people speak it fluently.[3]
Though no longer an official language in the Philippines, Philippine Spanish has had a tremendous influence on the native tongues of the archipelago, including Filipino.
The Spanish spoken in Gibraltar is essentially not different from the neighboring areas in Spain, except for code-switching with English and some unique vocabulary items. It is frequently blended with English as a sort of Spanglish known as Llanito.
Judaeo-Spanish, a "Jewish language", encompasses a number of linguistic varieties based mostly on 15th-century Spanish; it is still spoken in a few small communities, mainly in Israel, but also in Turkey and a number of other countries.[4] As Jews have migrated since their expulsion from Iberia, the language has picked up several loan words from other languages and developed unique forms of spelling, grammar, and syntax. It can be considered either a very divergent dialect of Spanish, retaining features from Old Spanish,[5] or a separate language.
See main article: Phonological history of Spanish coronal fricatives. The distinction between pronounced as //s// and pronounced as //θ// is maintained in northern Spain (in all positions) and in south-central Spain (only in syllable onset), while the two phonemes are not distinguished in the Americas, the Canary Islands, the Philippines and much of Andalusia. The maintenance of phonemic contrast is called distinción in Spanish. In areas that do not distinguish them, they are typically realized as pronounced as /[s]/, though in parts of southern Andalusia the realization is closer to pronounced as /[θ]/; in Spain uniform use of pronounced as /[θ]/ is called ceceo and uniform use of [s] seseo.
In dialects with seseo the words ('house') and ('hunt') are pronounced as homophones (generally pronounced as /[ˈkasa]/), whereas in dialects with distinción they are pronounced differently (as pronounced as /[ˈkasa]/ and pronounced as /[ˈkaθa]/ respectively). The symbol pronounced as /[s]/ stands for a voiceless sibilant like the s of English sick, while pronounced as /[θ]/ represents a voiceless interdental fricative like the th of English think.
In some cases where the phonemic merger would render words homophonic in the Americas, one member of the pair is frequently replaced by a synonym or derived form—e.g. caza replaced by, or ('to boil'), homophonic with ('to sew'), replaced by . For more on seseo, see González-Bueno.
See main article: Yeísmo. Traditionally Spanish had a phonemic distinction between pronounced as //ʎ// (a palatal lateral approximant, written ll) and pronounced as //ʝ// (a palatal approximant, written y). But for most speakers in Spain and the Americas, these two phonemes have been merged in the phoneme pronounced as //ʝ//. This merger results in the words ('silenced') and ('fell') being pronounced the same, whereas they remain distinct in dialects that have not undergone the merger. The use of the merged phoneme is called "yeísmo".
In Spain, the distinction is preserved in some rural areas and smaller cities of the north, while in South America the contrast is characteristic of bilingual areas where Quechua languages and other indigenous languages that have the pronounced as //ʎ// sound in their inventories are spoken (this is the case of inland Peru and Bolivia), and in Paraguay.
The phoneme pronounced as //ʝ// can be pronounced in a variety of ways, depending on the dialect. In most of the area where yeísmo is present, the merged phoneme pronounced as //ʝ// is pronounced as the approximant pronounced as /link/, and also, in word-initial positions, an affricate pronounced as /link/. In the area around the Río de la Plata (Argentina, Uruguay), this phoneme is pronounced as a palatoalveolar sibilant fricative, either as voiced pronounced as /link/ or, especially by young speakers, as voiceless pronounced as /link/.
One of the most distinctive features of the Spanish variants is the pronunciation of pronounced as //s// when it is not aspirated to pronounced as /[h]/ or elided. In northern and central Spain, and in the Paisa Region of Colombia, as well as in some other, isolated dialects (e.g. some inland areas of Peru and Bolivia), the sibilant realization of pronounced as //s// is an apico-alveolar retracted fricative pronounced as /link/, a sound transitional between laminodental pronounced as /link/ and palatal pronounced as /link/. However, in most of Andalusia, in a few other areas in southern Spain, and in most of Latin America it is instead pronounced as a lamino-alveolar or dental sibilant. The phoneme pronounced as //s// is realized as pronounced as /link/ or pronounced as /link/ before voiced consonants when it is not aspirated to pronounced as /[h]/ or elided; pronounced as /link/ is a sound transitional between pronounced as /[z]/ and pronounced as /link/. Before voiced consonants, [{{IPA|z}} ~ {{IPA|z̺}}] is more common in natural and colloquial speech and oratorical pronunciation, [s ~ {{IPA|s̺}}] is mostly pronounced in emphatic and slower speech.
In the rest of the article, the distinction is ignored and the symbols (IPA|s z) are used for all alveolar fricatives.
In much of Latin America—especially in the Caribbean and in coastal and lowland areas of Central and South America—and in the southern half of Spain, syllable-final pronounced as //s// is either pronounced as a voiceless glottal fricative, pronounced as /[h]/ (debuccalization, also frequently called "aspiration"), or not pronounced at all. In some varieties of Latin American Spanish (notably Honduran and Salvadoran Spanish) this may also occur intervocalically within an individual word—as with, which may be pronounced as pronounced as /[noˈhotɾoh]/—or even in initial position. In southeastern Spain (eastern Andalusia, Murcia and part of La Mancha), the distinction between syllables with a now-silent s and those originally without s is preserved by pronouncing the syllables ending in s with pronounced as /[{{IPAplink|æ}}, {{IPAplink|ɛ}}, {{IPAplink|ɔ}}]/ (that is, the open/closed syllable contrast has been turned into a tense/lax vowel contrast); this typically affects the vowels pronounced as //a//, pronounced as //e// and pronounced as //o//, but in some areas even pronounced as //i// and pronounced as //u// are affected, turning into pronounced as /[{{IPAplink|ɪ}}, {{IPAplink|ʊ}}]/. For instance, ('all the swans are white'), can be pronounced pronounced as /[ˈtoðoh loh ˈθihne(s) som ˈblaŋkoh]/, or even pronounced as /[ˈtɔðɔ lɔ ˈθɪɣnɛ som ˈblæŋkɔ]/ (Standard Peninsular Spanish: pronounced as /[ˈtoðoz los ˈθizne(s) som ˈblaŋkos]/, Latin American Spanish: pronounced as /[ˈtoðoz lo(s) ˈsizne(s) som ˈblaŋkos]/). This vowel contrast is sometimes reinforced by vowel harmony, so that casas pronounced as /[ˈkæsæ]/ 'houses' differs from casa pronounced as /[ˈkasa]/ not only by the lack of the final pronounced as /[s]/ in the former word but also in the quality of both of the vowels. For those areas of southeastern Spain where the deletion of final pronounced as //s// is complete, and where the distinction between singular and plural of nouns depends entirely on vowel quality, the case has been made to claim that a set of phonemic splits has occurred, resulting in a system with eight vowel phonemes in place of the standard five.
In the dialects that feature s-aspiration, it works as a sociolinguistic variable, [h] being more common in natural and colloquial speech, whereas [s] tends to be pronounced in emphatic and slower speech. In oratorical pronunciation, it depends on the country and speaker; if the Spanish speaker chooses to pronounces all or most of syllable-final [s], it is mostly voiced to [{{IPA|z}}] before voiced consonants.
Although the vowels of Spanish are relatively stable from one dialect to another, the phenomenon of vowel reduction—devoicing or even loss—of unstressed vowels in contact with voiceless consonants, especially pronounced as //s//, can be observed in the speech of central Mexico (including Mexico City). For example, it can be the case that the words ('pesos [money]'), ('weights'), and ('fish [pl.]') sound nearly the same, as pronounced as /[ˈpesː]/. One may hear ('well (then)') pronounced pronounced as /[ps̩]/. Some efforts to explain this vowel reduction link it to the strong influence of Nahuatl and other Native American languages in Mexican Spanish.
In the 16th century, as the Spanish colonization of the Americas was beginning, the phoneme now represented by the letter j had begun to change its place of articulation from palato-alveolar pronounced as /[ʃ]/ to palatal pronounced as /[ç]/ and to velar pronounced as /[x]/, like German ch in Bach (see History of Spanish and Old Spanish language). In southern Spanish dialects and in those Hispanic American dialects strongly influenced by southern settlers (e.g. Caribbean Spanish), rather than the velar fricative pronounced as /[x]/, the sound was backed all the way to pronounced as /[h]/, like English h in hope. Glottal pronounced as /[h]/ is nowadays the standard pronunciation for j in Caribbean dialects (Cuban, Dominican, and Puerto Rican) as well as in mainland Venezuela, in most Colombian dialects excepting Pastuso dialect that belongs to a continuum with Ecuadorian Spanish, much of Central America, southern Mexico, the Canary Islands, Extremadura and western Andalusia in Spain; in the rest of the country, pronounced as /[x]/ alternates with a "raspy" uvular fricative pronounced as /[χ]/, sometimes pronounced with a simultaneous voiceless uvular trill. In the rest of the Americas, the velar fricative pronounced as /[x]/ is prevalent. In Chile, pronounced as //x// is fronted to pronounced as /link/ (like German ch in ich) when it precedes the front vowels pronounced as //i// and pronounced as //e//: pronounced as /[ˈçente]/, pronounced as /[çiˈnete]/; in other phonological environments it is pronounced pronounced as /[x]/ or [h].
For the sake of simplicity, these are given a broad transcription (IPA|x) in the rest of the article.
In standard European Spanish, as well as in many dialects in the Americas (e.g. standard Argentine or Rioplatense, inland Colombian, and Mexican), word-final pronounced as //n// is, by default (i.e. when followed by a pause or by an initial vowel in the following word), alveolar, like English pronounced as /[n]/ in pen. When followed by a consonant, it assimilates to that consonant's place of articulation, becoming dental, interdental, palatal, or velar. In some dialects, however, word-final pronounced as //n// without a following consonant is pronounced as a velar nasal pronounced as /[ŋ]/ (like the -ng of English long), and may produce vowel nasalization. In these dialects, words such as ('bread') and ('well') may sound like pang and byeng to English-speakers. Velar -n is common in many parts of Spain (Galicia, León, Asturias, Murcia, Extremadura, Andalusia, and Canary Islands). In the Americas, velar -n is prevalent in all Caribbean dialects, Central American dialects, the coastal areas of Colombia, Venezuela, much of Ecuador, Peru, and northern Chile.This velar -n likely originated in the northwest of Spain, and from there spread to Andalusia and then the Americas.[6] Loss of final -n with strong nasalization of the preceding vowel is not infrequent in all those dialects where velar -n exists. In much of Ecuador, Peru, Venezuela (except for the Andean region) and Dominican Spanish, any pre-consonantal nasal can be realized pronounced as /[ŋ]/; thus, a word like ambientación can be pronounced pronounced as /[aŋbjeŋtaˈsjoŋ]/.
All varieties of Spanish distinguish between a "single-R" and a "double-R" phoneme. The single-R phoneme corresponds to the letter r written once (except when word-initial or following l, n, or s) and is pronounced as pronounced as /[ɾ]/, an alveolar flap—like American English tt in better—in virtually all dialects. The single-R/double-R contrast is neutralized in syllable-final position, and in some dialects these phonemes also lose their contrast with pronounced as //l//, so a word such as may sound like altesanía. This neutralization or "leveling" of coda pronounced as //r// and pronounced as //l// is frequent in dialects of southern Spain, the Caribbean, Venezuela and coastal Colombia.
The double-R phoneme is spelled rr between vowels (as in carro 'car') and r word-initially (e.g. rey 'king', ropa 'clothes') or following l, n, or s (e.g. alrededor 'around', enriquecer 'enrich', enrollar 'roll up', enrolar 'enroll', honra 'honor', Conrado 'Conrad', Israel 'Israel'). In most varieties it is pronounced as an alveolar trill pronounced as /[r]/, and that is considered the prestige pronunciation. Two notable variants occur additionally: one sibilant and the other velar or uvular. The trill is also found in lexical derivations (morpheme-initial positions), and prefixation with sub and ab: abrogado pronounced as /[aβroˈɣa(ð)o]/, 'abrogated', subrayar pronounced as /[suβraˈʝar]/, 'to underline'. The same goes for the compound word ciudadrealeño (from Ciudad Real). However, after vowels, the initial r of the root becomes rr in prefixed or compound words: prorrogar, infrarrojo, autorretrato, arriesgar, puertorriqueño, Monterrey. In syllable-final position, inside a word, the tap is more frequent, but the trill can also occur (especially in emphatic or oratorical style) with no semantic difference, especially before l, m, n, s, t, or d—thus arma ('weapon') may be either pronounced as /[ˈaɾma]/ (tap) or pronounced as /[ˈarma]/ (trill), perla ('pearl') may be either pronounced as /[ˈpeɾla]/ or pronounced as /[ˈperla]/, invierno ('winter') may be pronounced as /[imˈbjeɾno]/ or pronounced as /[imˈbjerno]/, verso ('verse') may be pronounced as /[ˈbeɾso]/ or pronounced as /[ˈberso]/, and verde ('green') pronounced as /[ˈbeɾðe]/ or pronounced as /[ˈberðe]/. In word-final position the rhotic will usually be: either a trill or a tap when followed by a consonant or a pause, as in amopronounced as /[r ~ ɾ]/ paterno 'paternal love') and amopronounced as /[r ~ ɾ]/, with the tap being more frequent and the trill before l, m, n, s, t, d, or sometimes a pause; or a tap when followed by a vowel-initial word, as in amopronounced as /[ɾ]/ eterno 'eternal love') (Can be a trill or tap with a temporary glottal stop in emphatic speech: amopronounced as /[rʔ ~ ɾʔ]/ eterno, with trill being more common). Morphologically, a word-final rhotic always corresponds to the tapped pronounced as /[ɾ]/ in related words. Thus, the word olor 'smell' is related to olores, oloroso 'smells, smelly' and not to *olorres, *olorroso, and the word taller 'workshop' is related to talleres 'workshops' and not to *tallerres.[7] When two rhotics occur consecutively across a word or prefix boundary, they result in one trill, so that da rosas ('s/he gives roses') and dar rosas ('give roses') are either neutralized, or distinguished by a longer trill in the latter phrase, which may be transcribed as pronounced as /[rr]/ or pronounced as /[rː]/ (although this is transcribed with (IPA|ɾr) in, even though it differs from pronounced as /[r]/ purely by length); da rosas and dar rosas may be distinguished as pronounced as /[da ˈrosas]/ vs. pronounced as /[darˈrosas]/, or they may fall together as the former.[8]
The pronunciation of the double-R phoneme as a voiced strident (or sibilant) apical fricative is common in New Mexico, Guatemala, Costa Rica, Ecuador, Bolivia, Chile, and Paraguay; in western and northern Argentina; and among older speakers in highland areas of Colombia. Some linguists have attempted to explain the assibilated rr (written in IPA as [{{IPA link|r̝}}]) as a result of influence from Native American languages, and it is true that in the Andean regions mentioned an important part of the population is bilingual in Spanish and one or another indigenous language. Nonetheless, other researchers have pointed out that sibilant rr in the Americas may not be an autonomous innovation, but rather a pronunciation that originated in some northern Spanish dialects and then was exported to the Americas. Spanish dialects spoken in the Basque Country, Navarre, La Rioja, and northern Aragon (regions that contributed substantially to Spanish-American colonization) show the fricative or postalveolar variant for rr (especially for the word-initial rr sound, as in Roma or rey). This is also pronounced voiceless when the consonants after the trill are voiceless and speaking in emphatic speech; it is written as [{{IPA|r̝̊}}], it sounds like a simultaneous pronounced as /[r]/ and pronounced as /[ʃ]/. In Andean regions, the alveolar trill is realized as an alveolar approximant pronounced as /[ɹ]/ or even as a voiced apico-alveolar pronounced as /link/, and it is quite common in inland Ecuador, Peru, most of Bolivia and in parts of northern Argentina and Paraguay. The alveolar approximant realization is particularly associated with the substrate of Native American languages, as is the assibilation of pronounced as //ɾ// to pronounced as /link/ in Ecuador and Bolivia. Assibilated trill is also found in dialects in the /sr/ sequence wherein /s/ is unaspirated, example: las rosas pronounced as /[la ˈr̝osas]/ ('the roses'), Israel pronounced as /[iˈr̝ael]/. The assibilated trill in this example is sometimes pronounced voiceless in emphatic and slower speech: las rosas pronounced as /[la ˈr̝̊osas]/ ('the roses'), Israel pronounced as /[iˈr̝̊ael]/.The other major variant for the rr phoneme—common in Puerto Rico and the Dominican Republic—is articulated at the back of the mouth, either as a glottal pronounced as /[h]/ followed by a voiceless apical trill [{{IPA link|r̥}}] or, especially in Puerto Rico, with a posterior articulation that ranges variously from a velar fricative pronounced as /link/ to a uvular trill pronounced as /link/. Canfield describes it as a voiceless uvular trill pronounced as /link/. These realizations for rr maintain their contrast with the phoneme pronounced as //x//, as the latter tends to be realized as a soft glottal pronounced as /[h]/: compare Ramón pronounced as /[xaˈmoŋ] ~ [ʀ̥aˈmoŋ]/ ('Raymond') with jamón pronounced as /[haˈmoŋ]/ ('ham').
In Puerto Rico, syllable-final pronounced as //r// can be realized as pronounced as /link/ (probably an influence of American English), aside from pronounced as /[ɾ]/, pronounced as /[r]/, and pronounced as /[l]/, so that verso ('verse') becomes pronounced as /[ˈbeɹso]/, alongside pronounced as /[ˈbeɾso]/, pronounced as /[ˈberso]/, or pronounced as /[ˈbelso]/; invierno ('winter') becomes pronounced as /[imˈbjeɹno]/, alongside pronounced as /[imˈbjeɾno]/, pronounced as /[imˈbjerno]/, or pronounced as /[imˈbjelno]/; and parlamento (parliament) becomes pronounced as /[paɹlaˈmento]/, alongside pronounced as /[paɾlaˈmento]/, pronounced as /[parlaˈmento]/, or pronounced as /[palaˈmento]/. In word-final position, the realization of pronounced as //r// depends on whether it is followed by a consonant-initial word or a pause, on the one hand, or by a vowel-initial word, on the other:
The same situation happens in Belize and the Archipelago of San Andrés, Providencia and Santa Catalina, in these cases an influence of British English.
Although in most Spanish-speaking territories and regions, guttural or uvular realizations of /r/ are considered a speech defect, back variants for pronounced as //r// (pronounced as /[ʀ]/, pronounced as /[x]/ or pronounced as /[χ]/) are widespread in rural Puerto Rican Spanish and in the dialect of Ponce,[9] whereas they are heavily stigmatized in the dialect of the capital San Juan.[10] To a lesser extent, velar variants of pronounced as //r// are found in some rural Cuban (Yateras, Guantánamo Province)[11] and Dominican vernaculars (Cibao, eastern rural regions of the country).[12]
In Paraguay, syllable-final pronounced as //r// is pronounced as pronounced as /link/ before l or s and word-final position, influenced by a substrate from Native American languages.
In Chile, as in Andalusia, the archiphoneme pronounced as //r// in the sequence pronounced as //rn// is sometimes assimilated to pronounced as /[nn]/ in lower-class speakers, and sometimes in educated speakers. Thus, jornada pronounced as //xorˈnada// 'workday' may be pronounced pronounced as /[xonˈnaː]/.
Additionally, in the Basque-speaking areas of Spain, the uvular articulation for pronounced as //r//, pronounced as /[ʁ]/, has a higher prevalence among bilinguals than among Spanish monolinguals.[13]
The letter x usually represents the phoneme sequence pronounced as //ɡs//. An exception to this is the pronunciation of the x in some place names, especially in Mexico, such as and the name itself, reflecting an older spelling (see "Name of Mexico"). Some personal names, such as Javier, Jiménez, Rojas, etc., also are occasionally spelled with X: Xavier, Ximénez, Roxas, etc., where the letter is pronounced pronounced as //x//. A small number of words in Mexican Spanish retain the historical pronounced as //ʃ// pronunciation, e.g. .
There are two possible pronunciations of pronounced as //ɡs// in standard speech: the first one is pronounced as /[ks]/, with a voiceless plosive, but it is commonly realized as pronounced as /[ɣs]/ instead (hence the phonemic transcription pronounced as //ɡs//). Voicing is not contrastive in the syllable coda.
In dialects with seseo, c following x pronounced pronounced as //ɡs// is deleted, yielding pronunciations such as pronounced as /[eɣseˈlente, ek-]/ for .
Mexican Spanish and some other Latin American dialects have adopted from the native languages the voiceless alveolar affricate pronounced as /[ts]/ and many words with the cluster pronounced as /[tl]/ (originally an affricate pronounced as /[tɬ]/) represented by the respective digraphs (tz) and (tl), as in the names Azcapotzalco and Tlaxcala. pronounced as //tl// is a valid onset cluster in Latin America, with the exception of Puerto Rico, in the Canary Islands, and in the northwest of Spain, including Bilbao and Galicia. In these dialects, words of Greek and Latin origin with (tl), such as and, are also pronounced with onset pronounced as //tl//: pronounced as /[aˈtlantiko]/, pronounced as /[aˈtleta]/. In other dialects, the corresponding phonemic sequence is pronounced as //dl// (where pronounced as //l// is the onset), with the coda pronounced as //d// realized variously as pronounced as /link/ and pronounced as /link/. The usual pronunciation of those words in most of Spain is pronounced as /[aðˈlantiko]/ and pronounced as /[aðˈleta]/.[14] [15]
The pronounced as /[ts]/ sound also occurs in European Spanish in loanwords of Basque origin (but only learned loanwords, not those inherited from Roman times), as in abertzale. In colloquial Castilian it may be replaced by pronounced as //tʃ// or pronounced as //θ//. In Bolivian, Paraguayan, and Coastal Peruvian Spanish, pronounced as /[ts]/ also occurs in loanwords of Japanese origin.
Spanish has a fricative pronounced as /[ʃ]/ for loanwords of origins from native languages in Mexican Spanish, loanwords of French, German and English origin in Chilean Spanish, loanwords of Italian, Galician, French, German and English origin in Rioplatense Spanish and Venezuelan Spanish, Chinese loanwords in Coastal Peruvian Spanish, Japanese loanwords in Bolivian Spanish, Paraguayan Spanish, Coastal Peruvian Spanish, Basque loanwords in Castilian Spanish (but only learned loanwords, not those inherited from Roman times), and English loanwords in Puerto Rican Spanish and all dialects.[16]
The Spanish digraph ch (the phoneme pronounced as //tʃ//) is pronounced pronounced as /link/ in most dialects. However, it is pronounced as a fricative pronounced as /link/ in some Andalusian dialects, New Mexican Spanish, some varieties of northern Mexican Spanish, informal and sometimes formal Panamanian Spanish, and informal Chilean Spanish. In Chilean Spanish this pronunciation is viewed as undesirable, while in Panama it occurs among educated speakers. In Madrid and among upper- and middle-class Chilean speakers, it can be pronounced as the alveolar affricate pronounced as /link/.
In some dialects of southeastern Spain (Murcia, eastern Andalusia and a few adjoining areas) where the weakening of final pronounced as //s// leads to its disappearance, the "silent" pronounced as //s// continues to have an effect on the preceding vowel, opening the mid vowels pronounced as //e// and pronounced as //o// to pronounced as /[ɛ]/ and pronounced as /[ɔ]/ respectively, and fronting the open central vowel pronounced as //a// toward pronounced as /[æ]/. Thus the singular/plural distinction in nouns and adjectives is maintained by means of the vowel quality:
Furthermore, this opening of final mid vowels can affect other vowels earlier in the word, as an instance of metaphony:
(In the remaining dialects, the mid vowels have nondistinctive open and closed allophones determined by the shape of the syllable or by contact with neighboring phonemes. See Spanish phonology.)
Final, non-stressed pronounced as //e// and pronounced as //o// may be raised to pronounced as /[i]/ and pronounced as /[u]/ respectively in some rural areas of Spain and Latin America. Examples include Spanish; Castilian: noche > nochi 'night', Spanish; Castilian: viejo > vieju. In Spain, this is mainly found in Galicia and other northern areas. This type of raising carries negative prestige.
See main article: Judaeo-Spanish.
Judaeo-Spanish (often called Ladino) refers to the Romance dialects spoken by Jews whose ancestors were expelled from Spain near the end of the 15th century.
These dialects have important phonological differences compared to varieties of Spanish proper; for example, they have preserved the voiced/voiceless distinction among sibilants as they were in Old Spanish. For this reason, the letter (s), when written single between vowels, corresponds to a voiced pronounced as /[z]/—e.g. pronounced as /[ˈroza]/ ('rose'). Where (s) is not between vowels and is not followed by a voiced consonant, or when it is written double, it corresponds to voiceless pronounced as /[s]/—thus pronounced as /[asenˈtarse]/ ('to sit down'). And due to a phonemic neutralization similar to the seseo of other dialects, the Old Spanish voiced (z) pronounced as /[dz]/ and the voiceless (ç) pronounced as /[ts]/ have merged, respectively, with pronounced as //z// and pronounced as //s//—while maintaining the voicing contrast between them. Thus ('to make') has gone from the medieval pronounced as /[faˈdzer]/ to pronounced as /[faˈzer]/, and ('town square') has gone from pronounced as /[ˈplatsa]/ to pronounced as /[ˈplasa]/.[17]
A related dialect is Haketia, the Judaeo-Spanish of northern Morocco. This too tended to assimilate with modern Spanish, during the Spanish occupation of the region.Tetuani Ladino was brought to Oran in Algeria.
Patterns of intonation differ significantly according to dialect, and native speakers of Spanish use intonation to quickly identify different accents. To give some examples, intonation patterns differ between Peninsular and Mexican Spanish, and also between northern Mexican Spanish and accents of the center and south of the country. Argentine Spanish is also characterized by its unique intonation patterns which are supposed to be influenced by the languages of Italy, particularly Neapolitan. Language contact can affect intonation as well, as the Spanish spoken in Cuzco and Mallorca show influence from Quechua and Catalan intonation patterns, respectively, and distinct intonation patterns are found in some ethnically homogenous Afro-Latino communities. Additionally, some scholars have historically argued that indigenous languages influenced the development of Latin America's regional intonation patterns.[18] [19] [20]
Spanish is a language with a "T–V distinction" in the second person, meaning that there are different pronouns corresponding to "you" which express different degrees of formality. In most varieties, there are two degrees, namely "formal" and "familiar" (the latter is also called "informal").
For the second person formal, virtually all Spanish dialects of Spain and the Americas use and (singular and plural respectively). But for the second person familiar, there is regional variation—between and for the singular, and, separately, between and for the plural. The use of vos (and its corresponding verb forms) rather than tú is called voseo.
Each of the second-person pronouns has its historically corresponding verb forms, used by most speakers. Most speakers use both the pronoun vos and its historically corresponding verb forms (e.g. vos , 'you have'). But some dialects use the pronoun tú with "vos verb forms" (verbal voseo—tú ), while others use vos with "tú verb forms" (pronominal voseo—vos ).
In most dialects the familiar second person singular pronoun is tú (from Latin tū), and the formal pronoun is usted (usually considered to originate from , meaning 'your grace' or, literally, 'your mercy'). In a number of regions in the Americas, tú is replaced by another pronoun, vos, and the verb conjugation changes accordingly (see details below). Spanish vos comes from Latin vōs, the second person plural pronoun in Latin.
In any case, there is wide variation as to when each pronoun (formal or familiar) is used. In Spain, tú is familiar (for example, used with friends), and usted is formal (for example, used with older people). In recent times, there has been a noticeable tendency to extend the use of tú even in situations previously reserved for usted. Meanwhile, in several countries (in parts of Middle America, especially, Costa Rica and Colombia), the formal usted is also used to denote a closer personal relationship. Many Colombians and some Chileans, for instance, use usted for a child to address a parent and also for a parent to address a child. Some countries, such as Cuba and the Dominican Republic, prefer the use of tú even in very formal circumstances, and usted is seldom used.
Meanwhile, in other countries, the use of formal rather than familiar second-person pronouns denotes authority. In Peru, for example, senior military officers use tú to speak to their subordinates, but junior officers use only usted to address their superior officers.
Using the familiar tú, especially in contexts where usted was to be expected, is called . The corresponding verb is (a transitive verb, the direct object being the person addressed with the pronoun). The verb tutear is used even in those dialects whose familiar pronoun is vos and means 'to treat with the familiar second-person pronoun'.
Pronominal voseo, the use of the pronoun vos instead of tú, is the prevalent form of the familiar second person singular pronoun in Guatemala, El Salvador, Honduras, Nicaragua, Costa Rica, Paraguay, Argentina and Uruguay. In those countries, it is used by many to address others in all kinds of contexts, often regardless of social status or age, including by cultured/educated speakers and writers, in television, advertisements, and even in translations from other languages. In Guatemala and Uruguay, vos and tú are used concurrently, but vos is much more common. Both pronouns use the verb forms normally associated with vos (vos / tú querés, 'you want').
The name Rioplatense is applied to the dialect of Spanish spoken around the mouth of the Río de la Plata and the lower course of the Paraná River, where vos, not tú, is invariably used, with the vos verb forms (vos tenés). The area comprises the most populous part of Argentina (the provinces of Buenos Aires and Santa Fe) as well as an important part of Uruguay, including Montevideo, the capital.
In Ecuador, vos is the most prominent form throughout the Sierra region of the country, though it does coexist with usted and the lesser-used tú. In this region, vos is regarded as the conversational norm, but it is not used in public discourse or the mass media. The choice of pronoun to be used depends on the participants' likeness in age and/or social status. Based on these factors, speakers can assess themselves as being equal, superior, or inferior to the addressee, and the choice of pronoun is made on this basis, sometimes resulting in a three-tiered system. Ecuadorians of the Highlands thus generally use vos among familiarized equals or by superiors (in both social status and age) to inferiors; tú among unfamiliarized equals, or by a superior in age but inferior in social status; and usted by both familiarized and unfamiliarized inferiors, or by a superior in social status but inferior in age. In the more populated coastal region, the form tú is used in most situations, usted being used only for unfamiliar and/or superior addressees.
Vos can be heard throughout most of Chile, Bolivia, and a small part of Peru as well, but in these places it is regarded as substandard. It is also used as the conversational norm in the Paisa Region and the southwest region of Colombia, in Zulia State (Venezuela), in Honduras, El Salvador, Costa Rica, Guatemala, and the state of Chiapas in Mexico.
In Chile, even though tú is the prestige pronoun among educated speakers, the use of "verbal voseo", i.e. "tú + verb conjugation of vos" (e.g. tú ) is widespread. On the other hand, "pronominal voseo", the use of the pronoun vos—pronounced with aspiration of the final pronounced as //s//—is used derisively in informal speech between close friends as playful banter (usually among men) or, depending on the tone of voice, as an offensive comment.
In Colombia, the choice of second person singular varies with location. In most of inland Colombia (especially the Andean region), usted is the pronoun of choice for all situations, even in speaking between friends or family; but in large cities (especially Bogotá), the use of tú is becoming more accepted in informal situations, especially between young interlocutors of opposite sexes and among young women. In Valle del Cauca (Cali), Antioquia (Medellín) and the Pacific coast, the pronouns used are vos and usted. On the Caribbean coast (mainly Barranquilla and Cartagena), tú is used for practically all informal situations and many formal situations as well, usted being reserved for the most formal environments. A peculiarity occurs in the Altiplano Cundiboyacense and among some speakers in Bogotá: usted is replaced by for formal situations (it is relatively easy to identify a Boyacense by his/her use of this pronoun). Sumercé comes from su merced ('your mercy').
In parts of Spain, a child used to use not tú but usted to address a parent. Today, however, this usage is unusual. Among the factors for the ongoing replacement of usted by tú are the new social relevance of youth and the reduction of social differences. In particular, it has been attributed to the egalitarianism of the right-wing Falange party. By contrast, Spanish leftists of the early 20th century would address their comrades as usted as a show of respect and workers' dignity.
According to Joan Coromines, by the 16th century, the use of vos (as a second person singular pronoun) had been reduced to rural areas of Spain, which were a source of many emigrants to the New World, and so vos became the unmarked form in many areas of Latin America.
A slightly different explanation is that in Spain, even if vos (as a singular) originally denoted the high social status of those who were addressed as such (monarchs, nobility, etc.), the people never used the pronoun themselves since there were few or no people above them in society. Those who used vos were people of the lower classes and peasants. When the waves of Spanish immigrants arrived to populate the New World, they primarily came from these lower classes. In the New World, wanting to raise their social status from what it was in Spain, they demanded to be addressed as vos.
Through the widespread use of vos in the Americas, the pronoun was transformed into an indicator of low status not only for the addresser but also for the addressee. Conversely, in Spain, vos is now considered a highly exalted archaism virtually confined to liturgy.
Speakers of Ladino still use vos as it was used in the Middle Ages, to address people higher on the social ladder. The pronoun usted had not been introduced to this dialect of Spanish when the Jews were expelled from Spain in 1492 so vos is still used in Ladino much as usted is used in modern Spanish.
A variant of usted, vusted, can be heard in Andean regions of South America. Other, less frequent forms analogous to usted are (short for ), and usía (from vuestra ).
There is a traditional assumption that Chilean and River Plate voseo verb forms are derived from those corresponding to vosotros. This assumption, however, has been challenged, in an article by —in the theoretical framework of classical generative phonology—as synchronically inadequate, on the grounds that it requires at least six different rules, including three monophthongization processes that lack phonological motivation. Alternatively, the article argues that the Chilean and River Plate voseo verb forms are synchronically derived from underlying representations that coincide with those corresponding to the non-honorific second person singular tú. The proposed theory requires the use of only one special rule in the case of Chilean voseo. This rule—along with other rules that are independently justified in the language—makes it possible to derive synchronically all Chilean and River Plate voseo verb forms in a straightforward manner. The article additionally solves the problem posed by the alternate verb forms of Chilean voseo such as the future indicative (e.g. vay a bailar 'will you dance?'), the present indicative forms of haber (habih and hai 'you have'), and the present indicative of ser (soi, eríh and erei 'you are'), without resorting to any ad hoc rules.
In Standard European Spanish the plural of tú is vosotros and the plural of usted is ustedes. In Hispanic America vosotros is not used, and the plural of both tú and usted is ustedes. This means that speaking to a group of friends a Spaniard will use vosotros, while a Latin American Spanish speaker will use ustedes. Although ustedes is semantically a second-person form, it is treated grammatically as a third-person plural form because it originates from the term ('your [pl.] mercies,' sing. vuestra merced).
The only vestiges of vosotros in the Americas are / in Papiamento and the use of vuestro/a in place of (ustedes) as second person plural possessive in the Cusco region of Peru.
In very formal contexts, however, the vosotros conjugation can still be found. An example is the Mexican national anthem, which contains such forms as and .
The plural of the Colombian (Cundi-Boyacense Plateau) sumercé is sumercés/susmercedes, from sus mercedes ('your mercies').
In some parts of Andalusia (the lands around the Guadalquivir river and western Andalusia), the usage is what is called ustedes-vosotros: the pronoun ustedes is combined with the verb forms for vosotros. However, this sounds extremely colloquial and most Andalusians prefer to use each pronoun with its correct form.
In Ladino, vosotros is still the only second person plural pronoun, since ustedes does not exist.
Each second-person pronoun has its historically corresponding verb forms. The formal usted and ustedes, although semantically second person, take verb forms identical with those of the third person, singular and plural respectively, since they are derived from the third-person expressions vuestra merced and vuestras mercedes ('your grace[s]'). The forms associated with the singular vos can generally be derived from those for the plural vosotros by deleting the palatal semivowel of the ending (vosotros habláis > vos hablás, 'you speak'; vosotros coméis > vos comés, 'you eat').
General statements about the use of voseo in different localities should be qualified by the note that individual speakers may be inconsistent in their usage, and that isoglosses rarely coincide with national borders. That said, a few assertions can be made:
As for the second person familiar plural, it can be said that northern and central Spain use vosotros and its verb forms (vosotros habláis, 'you [pl.] speak'), while the rest of the Spanish-speaking world merges the familiar and formal in ustedes (ustedes hablan). Usage in western Andalusia includes the use of ustedes with the traditional vosotros verb form (ustedes habláis).
In Ladino, the second-person pronouns are quite different from those of Spain and Latin America. The forms usted and ustedes had not yet appeared in 1492 when the Jews were expelled from Spain. Speakers of Ladino still use vos as it was used in the Middle Ages (as a singular) to address people higher on the social ladder. And vosotros is the only second person plural pronoun. In Ladino the formal singular for 'you speak' is vos avláis (pronounced pronounced as /[aˈvlaʃ]/, and the same verb form serves for the plural, both formal and familiar: vosotros avláis (pronounced as /[voˈzotros aˈvlaʃ]/). The subjunctive 'that you lose (formal singular)' is que vos pedráis (pronounced as /[ke vos peˈdraʃ]/), while the plural (both formal and familiar) is que vosotros pedráis (pronounced as /[ke voˈzotros peˈdraʃ]/). The formal singular imperative ('come') is venid or vení, and the same form serves as the plural imperative, both formal and familiar.
See main article: Leísmo and Loísmo. In many dialects in northern and central Spain, including that of Madrid, the indirect object pronouns le and les may be used in place of the direct object pronouns lo, la, los, and las in a phenomenon known as leísmo. Leísmo typically occurs when the direct object refers to a person or personalized thing, such as a pet, and is most commonly used for male direct objects. The opposite phenomenon also occurs in the same regions of Spain and is known as loísmo or laísmo. In loísmo, the direct object pronouns lo and los are used in contexts where the indirect object pronouns le and les would normally be prescribed; this usually occurs with a male indirect object. In laísmo, la and las are used instead of le and les when referring to a female indirect object.
In a broad sense, when expressing an action viewed as finished in the past, speakers (and writers) in most of Spain use the perfect tense—e.g. he llegado *'I have arrived')—more often than their American counterparts, while Spanish-speakers in the Americas more often use the preterite (llegué 'I arrived').
The perfect is also called the "present perfect" and, in Spanish, pasado perfecto or pretérito perfecto compuesto. It is described as a "compound" tense (compuesto in Spanish) because it is formed with the auxiliary verb haber plus a main verb.
The preterite, also called the "simple past" and, in Spanish, pretérito indefinido or pretérito perfecto simple, is considered a "simple" tense because it is formed of a single word: the verb stem with an inflectional ending for person, number, etc.
The choice between preterite and perfect, according to prescriptive grammars from both Spain and the Americas, is based on the psychological time frame—whether expressed or merely implied—in which the past action is embedded. If that time frame includes the present moment (i.e. if the speaker views the past action as somehow related to the moment of speaking), then the recommended tense is the perfect (he llegado). But if the time frame does not include the present—if the speaker views the action as only in the past, with little or no relation to the moment of speaking—then the recommended tense is the preterite (llegué). This is also the real spontaneous usage in most of Spain.
Following this criterion, an explicit time frame such as hoy ('today') or este año ('this year') includes the present and thus dictates the compound tense: Este año he cantado ('I have sung this year'). Conversely, a time frame such as ayer ('yesterday') or la semana pasada ('last week') does not include the present and therefore calls for the preterite: La semana pasada canté ('I sang last week').
However, in most of the Americas, and in the Canary Islands, the preterite is used for all actions viewed as completed in the past. It tends to be used in the same way in those parts of Spain where the local languages and vernaculars do not have compound tenses, that is, the Galician-speaking area and the neighbouring Astur-Leonese-speaking area.
In most of Spain, the compound tense is preferred in most cases when the action described is close to the present moment:
Prescriptive norms would rule out the compound tense in a cuando-clause, as in the second example above.
Meanwhile, in Galicia, León, Asturias, Canary Islands and the Americas, speakers follow the opposite tendency, using the simple past tense in most cases, even if the action takes place at some time close to the present:
For some speakers of Latin American Spanish, the compound tense can sound affected, bookish, or foreign.
In Latin America one could say, "he viajado a España varias veces" ('I have traveled to Spain several times'), to express a repeated action, as in English. But to say El año pasado he viajado a España would sound ungrammatical (as it would also be in English to say "last year, I have traveled to Spain", as last year implies that the relevant time period does not include the present). In Spain, speakers use the compound tense when the period of time considered has not ended, as in he comprado un coche este año ('I have bought a car this year'). Meanwhile, a Latin American Spanish speaker is more likely to say, "compré un carro este año" ('I bought a car this year').
Different regional varieties of Spanish vary in terms of vocabulary as well. This includes both words that exist only in certain varieties (especially words borrowed from indigenous languages of the Americas), and words that are used differently in different areas. Among words borrowed from indigenous languages are many names for food, plants and animals, clothes, and household object, such as the following items of Mexican Spanish vocabulary borrowed from Nahuatl.
Word | English translation | |
---|---|---|
camote | sweet potato | |
pipián | stew | |
chapulín | grasshopper | |
huipil | blouse | |
metate | grinder, mortar and pestle |
In addition to loan words, there are a number of Spanish words that have developed distinct senses in different regional dialects. That is, for certain words a distinct meaning, either in addition to the standard meaning or in place of it, exists in some varieties of Spanish.
Word | Standard meaning | Regional meaning | |
---|---|---|---|
almacén | warehouse, department store | grocery store (Rioplatense Spanish, Chilean Spanish, Andean Spanish) | |
colectivo | collective | bus (Argentine Spanish originally 'collective taxi', Chilean Spanish, Bolivian Spanish)) | |
cuadra | stable, pigsty | city block (United States Spanish) | |
chaqueta | jacket | (vulgar) male masturbation (Central American Spanish) | |
coger | to take, to catch, to start, to feel | (vulgar) to fuck, have sexual relations (Rioplatense Spanish and Mexican Spanish) | |
concha | shell, tortoiseshell | (vulgar) cunt (Rioplatense Spanish, Chilean Spanish, Andean Spanish) | |
peloteo | knock-up (in tennis), warm up | fawning, adulation (Peninsular Spanish) |
The different dialects and accents do not block cross-understanding among relatively educated speakers. Meanwhile, the basilects have diverged more. The unity of the language is reflected in the fact that early imported sound films were dubbed into one version for the entire Spanish-speaking market. Currently, films not originally in Spanish (usually Hollywood productions) are typically dubbed separately into two or sometimes three accents: one for Spain, and for the Americas, either just one (Mexican), or two: a Mexican one for most of the Americas (using a neutral standardized accent without regionalisms) and one in Rioplatense Spanish for Argentina, Uruguay, and Paraguay. Some high-budget productions, however, such as the Harry Potter film series, have had dubs in three or more of the major accents. On the other hand, productions from another Spanish-language country are seldom dubbed. Exceptionally, the made-in-Spain animated features Dogtanian and the Three Muskehounds and The World of David the Gnome, as well as TV serials from the Andean countries such as Karkú (Chile), have had a Mexican dub. The popularity of telenovelas and music familiarizes the speakers with other accents of Spanish.
Prescription and a common cultural and literary tradition, among other factors, have contributed to the formation of a loosely defined register which can be termed Standard Spanish (or "Neutral Spanish"), which is the preferred form in formal settings, and is considered indispensable in academic and literary writing, the media, etc. This standard tends to disregard local grammatical, phonetic and lexical peculiarities, and draws certain extra features from the commonly acknowledged canon, preserving (for example) certain verb tenses considered "bookish" or archaic in most other dialects.
Mutual intelligibility in Spanish does not necessarily mean a translation is wholly applicable in all Spanish-speaking countries, especially when conducting health research that requires precision. For example, an assessment of the applicability of QWB-SA's Spanish version in Spain showed that some translated terms and usage applied US-specific concepts and regional lexical choices and cannot be successfully implemented without adaptation.[22]