Southeast Babar language explained

Southeast Babar
States:Indonesia
Region:Maluku
Speakers:4,500
Date:2007
Ref:e18
Familycolor:Austronesian
Fam2:Malayo-Polynesian (MP)
Fam3:Central–Eastern MP
Fam4:Babar
Fam5:South Babar
Fam6:Masela–Southeast
Iso3:vbb
Glotto:sout2883
Glottorefname:Southeast Babar

Southeast Babar is an Austronesian language spoken on Babar Island in South Maluku, Indonesia.[1]

Phonology

Consonants

The following consonant inventory is provided by Steinhauer (2009).

LabialAlveolarDorsal
Plosivevoicelessptk
voicedbd(ɡ)
Nasalmn(ŋ)
Fricative(f)(s)x
Trillr
Laterall
Approximantwj

Other consonants in Taber's wordlist

A wordlist collected in a 1993 article by Mark Taber records words with extra distinct sounds that are not recorded nor accorded phonemic status in Steinhauer's study.

Consonants exclusively appearing in Taber's wordlist! Sound! Words with the sound! Comments
[utʰ] "dog, banana"Corresponds to word-final /tj/ clusters recorded by Steinhauer, e.g. Steinhauer records "dog" and "banana" as uty.
h[taˈha] "this", [ɛhɛlˈlei] "here", [ɛhɛllɛˈnei] "there", [hlil] "ear"Should have been deleted by regular sound laws. Might correspond to /x/ in some cases (like in "ear", which is recorded by Steinhauer as xlil)
ʔ[ˈtaʔanɛ] "that"Steinhauer records glottal stops as occurring non-phonemically at the starts of otherwise vowel-initial words.
d͡ʒ[d͡ʒai] "what?"

Vowels

FrontCentralBack
Closei iːu
Midɛ ɛːɔ ɔː
Opena aː

Phonotactics

Due to extensive syncope of vowels in both prefixes and original final syllables, Southeast Babar admits a wide variety of consonant clusters both in the onsets and codas of syllables. One stark example of permitted consonant clusters is xweapk "we (inclusive) speak".

Word-final clusters of a consonant followed by /j/ may be optionally subject to epenthesis, with a non-phonemic [ə] being inserted either between the two consonants or after them. Both schwa positions can be used by the same speaker in free variation.

Phonological history

Southeast Babar is notable for its drastic phonetic reshapings of inherited Austronesian vocabulary, with extensive consonant loss, unusual reflexes of surviving consonants, and syncope and apocope of vowels. Many of these changes are outlined and exemplified by Hein Steinhauer as follows:[2]

Changes to consonants

Consonant loss

Elision of consonants in all historical positions is extensive throughout Southeast Babar. Ancestral Proto-Malayo-Polynesian sounds like *p, *k, *q, *R, *h *j and *z were simply lost in Southeast Babar with some exceptions.Examples of consonant deletions include:

Chain shift of *s, *t, and *k

The loss of *k led to a pull chain shift. Subsequently, *t shifted to k except if the *t was directly preceded by original *n; unshifted *t before *n is known from *punti > uty "banana". In turn, k produced from *t lenited to /x/ unless the *k was:

Lenited reflexes of *t > k > x include *teliŋa > xlil "ear", *mata > mox "eye", *ma-qitəm > mexm "black", *matay > -moxy "to die", *batu > waxy "stone", and *(h)əpat > wo-ax "4".

Afterwards, *s subsequently underwent fortition to t, with examples including:

Merger of *n, and *l

The inherited Malayo-Polynesian nasal consonants *n and merge with each other as *n, followed by a merger of that merged phoneme with *l, generally surfacing as /l/.

Post-merger /l/ subsequently underwent an inverse development to n when adjacent to t either originating from *s or borrowed from another language. This circular development leads to roots and inflectional affixes to synchronically contain alternations between l and n.

A few cases of n failing to merge with l are known, mainly in monosyllabic words where, due to medial consonant deletion and resulting vowel coalescence, there is simultaneously there is one n in the onset and a second n in the coda.

Due to *nipən "tooth" surfacing as lil and not **nin, Steinhauer suggests that the loss of *p occurred after the loss of *j and *k.

Other consonants

Changes to vowels

Reduction and loss of final vowels

All word-final and *a, whether inherited as word-final or secondarily word-final due to the loss of a following consonant, are deleted in Southeast Babar.

Word-final high vowels *-u and *-i generally reduce to the glide /j/. Like with *a, the loss of a following consonant will make the high vowel count as word-final for the purposes of this reduction.

Syncope of vowels between two consonants

Vowels often faced deletion in final syllables between two surviving consonants. Examples cited by Steinhauer include laŋit > lalk "heavens", *daləm > ralm "inside", *inum > *imun > -iml "to drink", *panas > pant "warm", and *matay > -moxy "to die".

Reflexes of *a

If an *a is either not word-initial, not preceded by a nasal, or not subject to apocope or syncope, it will remain as a.

Reflexes of other vowels

Morphology

Morphophonological processes

Some conditioned sound changes have led to phonetic mutations of morphemes when subject to morphological processes, including affixation and reduplication.

Change of l to n before t

Southeast Babar morphemes ending in l generally change the l to n whenever it precedes a t.

Glide metathesis and insertion

On verbs whose roots end in the glide -y, the glide and the first consonant of the following morpheme undergo metathesis. For instance, l-moxy "(s)he dies" becomes lmox tyel "(s)he has died", with the y from the verb root and the t of the particle tel switching places.

The metathesis process also occurs when a morpheme ending in -y is reduplicated, such as -kary "to work" reduplicating to -karkyary "to be working".

The 1st-person singular, 2nd-person singular, and 2nd-person plural also trigger the insertion of a glide after the first consonant of the following verb stem; the presence of glide insertion is governed by the same restrictions as the application of glide metathesis.

Glide metathesis and insertion have some restrictions on their application.

If metathesis is blocked by one of these two restrictions, the glide is simply lost unless a vowel follows the glide.

If an original Malayo-Polynesian *a was rounded to o after a nasal in a base morpheme, the vowel is reverted to a if the nasal is subject to the glide metathesis or insertion process. The conjugation of -moly "to laugh" demonstrates this:

Verb conjugation

Southeast Babar verbs are conjugated for three grammatical persons (first, second, and third persons) and two grammatical numbers (singular and plural). First-person plural conjugation also distinguishes clusivity, with exclusive 1st-person plural excluding the addressee but the inclusive 1st-person plural including them. In addition, verbs can additionally inflect for at least three known grammatical aspects, namely perfect, progressive aspect, and inchoative aspect.

Person-number prefixes

Southeast Babar conjugates verbs for person and number via a series of prefixes attached to verb stems, either to an unmarked stem or the progressive stem.

There are two basic classes of person-number prefixes in the language. One class has the person-number prefixes all contain a vowel, and the other class where most of the prefixes do not. The prefixes for each class are as follows:

Southeast Babar person-number verbal markers
Person and numberVocalic prefixesConsonantal prefixesProto-forms[3]
1st sg.o-i- [4]
  • ku-
2nd sg.mo-m-
  • mu-
3rd sg.le-l- [5]
  • na-
1st pl. inclusiveke-x-
  • ta-
1st pl. exclusiveme-m-
  • ma-
2nd pl.mi-m-
  • mi-
3rd pl.te-t-
  • sida-

Verbal aspect marking

A particle tel follows the conjugated verb to mark the perfect aspect. Another postverbal particle kay marks the inchoative aspect. The two particles can stack together to form an "inchoative perfect", so to speak. Steinbauer demonstrates the two particles with the following examples.

Progressive aspect is expressed by a special verbal stem for each verb formed via reduplication of the verb root.

Pronouns

The basic personal pronouns in Southeast Babar are:

Southeast Babar pronouns
singularplural
1st
person
exclusiveumam
inclusiveixy
2nd person(y)owmiy
3rd personiyity

Possessive markers

There are also at least two series of possessive markers, one series ending in u and another one ending in oːl. They all are stressed when attached to a possessed noun. Possessive markers documented by Steinhauer are first-person singular u and oːl, second-person singular mu and moːl, and first-person inclusive plural toːl.

Possessive markers follow the noun that is possessed. Examples of this behaviour include:

Numerals

Cardinal numerals from 2 to 9 have an obligatory prefix wu-/wo- that is derived from *buaq "fruit", which on the way to Southeast Babar evolved into a classifier and then a prefix.

The cardinals from 2-7 are given by Steinhauer as wu-ru "2", wo-kely "3", wo-ax "4", wu-lim "5", wo-lem "6", and wo-exy "7". Other numerals are only recorded in Taber's wordlist, including metl "1", wo-ka "8", wu-si "9", and wu-ki "10".

Notes and References

  1. Taber . Mark . 1993 . Toward a better understanding of the Indigenous Languages of Southwestern Maluku . Oceanic Linguistics . 32 . 2 . 389-441 . 3623199.
  2. Book: Steinhauer, Hein . The sounds of Southeast Babar . Canberra: Research School of Pacific and Asian Studies, Australian National University . 2009 . Adelaar, K. Alexander and Pawley, Andrew (eds.), Austronesian historical linguistics and culture history: a festschrift for Robert Blust . 399-409.
  3. Steinhauer posits that these proto-forms can only account directly for the consonantal prefixes, and finds it more difficult to envision how the vocalic prefixes can descend from these.
  4. Inserts a glide after a following consonant if this consonant is not w nor is followed by a high vowel.
  5. Surfaces as n- in verbs whose stem begins with t, like n-tol "(s)he sees", and also in the irregular verb n-oːn "(s)he eats".