Pilot decision making explained

Pilot decision making,[1] also known as aeronautical decision making (ADM),[2] is a process that aviators perform to effectively handle troublesome situations that are encountered. Pilot decision-making is applied in almost every stage of the flight as it considers weather, air spaces, airport conditions, estimated time of arrival and so forth. During the flight, employers pressure pilots regarding time and fuel restrictions since a pilots’ performance directly affects the company’s revenue and brand image. This pressure often hinders a pilot's decision-making process leading to dangerous situations as 50% to 90% of aviation accidents are the result of pilot error.[3] [4] [5]

Decision-making process

Since the 1980s,[6] the airline industry has identified the aeronautical decision-making (ADM) process as a critical factor in safe aeronautical operations. Airline industries are motivated to create decision-making procedures supplemented by crew resource management (CRM) to advance air safety.

thumb|A risk assessment checklist for pilots. This program includes a wide array of aviation-related activities specific to the pilot and assesses health, fatigue, weather, capabilities, etc. [7]

The pilot decision-making process is an effective five-step management skill that a pilot should conduct to maximize success chance when facing an unexpected or critical event. This cyclic model allows the pilot to make a critical decision and follow up with a series of events to produce the best possible resolution.

Mnemonics

Pilots use mnemonics to help them deal with emergencies and unexpected situations. One of the most famous mnemonics is the phrase "Aviate, Navigate, Communicate", to remind pilots what their priorities should be. The first priority is to keep the aircraft flying, avoiding undesired aircraft states and controlled flight into terrain. Next the pilot(s) should verify their location and navigate towards a suitable destination. Communication with air traffic control, while important, is a lower priority.[9]

Mnemonics used to decide and carry out a course of action include T-DODAR (Time, Diagnose, Options, Decision, Assign, Review),[10] [11] [12] [13] FOR-DEC (Facts, Options, Risks and benefits, Decide, Execute, Check),[14] [15] DECIDE (Detect, Estimate, Choose, Identify, Do, Evaluate),[16] DESIDE (Detect, Estimate, Set safety objectives, Identify, Do, Evaluate),[10] [17] [14] [18] GRADE (Gather Information, Review Information, Analyse Alternatives, Decide, Evaluate),[10] [19] 3P (Perceive, Process, Perform),[20] and PIOSEE (Problem, Information, Options, Select, Execute, Evaluate).[21] [22] FOR-DEC was developed by Lufthansa and the German Aerospace Center, and is used by numerous European airlines, as well as in German nuclear power plants.[23] The hyphen in FOR-DEC is designed to make the pilots stop and think about whether they have considered all the options.[23] T-DODAR is used by British Airways, who added the initial T to remind pilots to consider time available before starting the decision-making process.[10] [23] [24]

Advantages of these techniques include that they force the crew to name the facts; they prevent jumping to conclusions; they give co-pilots a means to make their voice heard; they allow both pilots to participate in the decision-making process; and they enable the captain to withdraw an incorrect decision without losing leadership authority. Disadvantages include that they can be an obstacle to quick and obvious actions;[10] they are used as a tool for justification rather than decision; that they don't provide a way to communicate non-communicable knowledge such as intuitions and "gut feelings". It is important that the technique used is standardised across an airline, so everyone is speaking the same language. It is important that the technique does not become an obstacle to solving problems.[23]

SHOR (Stimuli, Hypotheses, Options, Response) can be used in time-pressured situations.[23]

NITS can be used to brief during an emergency, for example to brief the cabin crew.[25] [26] [13]

Difficulties

Fatigue

Fatigue poses a significant issue in the aviation industry with the increase in demand for long-haul missions. Fatigue is especially detrimental to decision-making tasks, awareness-related tasks, and planning, which are the fundamental skills for pilots to operate their aircraft. This situation is especially dangerous since 26% of pilots deny the effect of fatigue. The official statistics showed a percentage of 4% to 8% of aviation accidents related to fatigue.[27] However, since fatigue lowers the performance of pilots and cripples their decision making process, fatigue impacts a much larger percentage of aviation accidents. The effects of fatigue are amplified with the changes in time zones due to jet lag disrupting biorhythm.

Fatigue related accidents

Pressure

During the flight, pilots are required to execute a specific departure and arrival time as the inability to meet these requirements results in the companies' increased fuel cost, delayed gate time fees, and delayed flights. These factors place pilots in a situation where their job performance directly correlates to the revenue of the employee company. This leads to high amounts of stress and pressure, which causes impairment in performance.[32]

There are significant difficulties presented during the phases associated with take-off and landing. The maneuvering process to approach and landing combined only accounts for 17% of the average flight time but is responsible for 70.2% of total aviation accidents.[33] Statistics prove a significantly larger number of accident occurrences during the phases where pilots are in stressed and pressured situations. At these phases, pilot decision-making can be critical. For example, the pilots of Asiana Airlines flight 214 were in a pressured and fatigued situation when they failed to overshoot after detecting a low approach path and high airspeed on the final approach.

Automation bias

The advancement in technology has enabled tasks that are too complex for humans and extended human capabilities. Automation such as GPS, traffic alert, and autopilot, has been incorporated into aviation and has become one of the prime resources for critical decision making. With the sophistication and accuracy of current technology, humans have been relying on it excessively, which results in automation bias. Referenced from Human-Computer Studies, an experiment was conducted to measure the effects of automation bias on decision making. Two control groups were selected to monitor a specific task, with the first group having access to reliable automation aid and the second group with no access to aid. The results showed that the second group in non-automated settings outperformed their counterpart. The first group made more errors when not explicitly prompted by automation, moreover, they followed the instruction of automation even when it contradicted their decision. This experiment shows the example of automation bias and participants' high degree of obedience to automation.[34] Automation bias can lead to critical errors in pilot decision making, as it is one of the many difficulties in today's digital age.

Weather decision

For the pilots flying under visual flight rule (VFR, in weather conditions clear enough to allow the pilot to see where the aircraft is going), it is significant to perform correct decision-making for the weather as they have to stay within the specific VFR weather requirements. The pilot must make a ‘go’ or ‘no-go’ decision as to if he or she will embark on a flight and if they will continue on the flight when the weather deteriorates.

VFR pilots primarily navigate by using the GPS, radio navigation systems, and most importantly pilotage. In order to perform pilotage, pilots must visually see the ground features and reference it to the map. Accidents are inevitable when weather conditions require pilots to fly primarily by reference to flight instruments without the proper instrument flight rules (IFR) equipments. In fact, over 19% of the general aviation crashes are caused from flying VFR in bad weather and 72% of these crashes are fatal.[35]

The research conducted by David O'Hare and Tracy Smitheram on pilots' decision-making in deteriorating conditions demonstrates the application of behavioral psychology to pilots. The experiment was conducted in a simulator where VFR pilots were presented with scenarios of cross-country flights in marginal weather. Participants of this experiment were measured by how their perspective of anticipated gains or losses affected the decision-making process. Results showed that the pilots who viewed decision making in the anticipated gains framework were significantly less likely to press on to deteriorating weather than the ones that were viewed in the losses framework.[36] This research shows that people are risk-averse when situations are viewed in terms of gains. It is important to compare the marginal benefit of pressing on into deteriorating weather to the risk associated with the flight to make the correct decision.

Commercial pilots and their associated airlines also have to contend with company expectations during their decision-making process regarding the weather. Commercial aircraft have higher capabilities for harsh weather, but their risk is significantly greater due to the passenger safety requirements and the sheer cost of the aircraft. Each airline has a different tolerance for weather, which poses problems for airlines that have more lenient protocols. Pilots are pressured to make a decision when canceling the flight, which could lead to a loss in reputation and revenue for the companies.

[37] [38] [39]

Emergencies

When pilots encounter emergencies, a checklist is referenced to follow a specific procedure to overcome the situation. However, not all parts of the emergency checklist explicitly state the qualitative actions that a pilot needs to perform. For example, in a forced landing, the pilot is required to choose a field to commit for landing, which requires the decision-making process to take into account winds, field quality, obstacles, distance, civilization, and other associated factors. The decision-making process is important as pilots are required to measure and compare the risks associated with each option. Four key conditions are required for an effective emergency decision.

It is important that if any of these conditions are absent, a defensive avoidance or hyper vigilance becomes prevalent and aggravates the decision making process. This theoretical model developed from psychological research provides a basis for pilots when confronting an emergency situation.[40]

See also

External links

Notes and References

  1. Web site: Pilot Decision Making — PDM - TP 13897 . Transport Canada . 23 June 2022.
  2. Book: Pilot's Handbook of Aeronautical Knowledge . 2016 . Federal Aviation Administration . 23 June 2022 . Chapter 2: Aeronautical Decision-Making.
  3. AERONAUTICAL DECISION-MAKING AND UNIVERSITYAVIATIONASSOCIATION CERTIFIED FLIGHT INSTRUCTORS. Bowman. Terry. 1994.
  4. Expertise Differences in Attentional Strategies Related to Pilot Decision Making. Human Factors: The Journal of the Human Factors and Ergonomics Society. 2008-12-01. 0018-7208. 19292010. 864–878. 50. 6. 10.1518/001872008X374974. Angela T.. Schriver. Daniel G.. Morrow. Christopher D.. Wickens. Donald A.. Talleur. 6513349.
  5. Web site: Ethical decision-making and the code of ethics of the Canadian Psychological Association.. APA PsycNET. 2015-10-31.
  6. Book: Kaempf . George L. . Klein . Gary . Aviation Psychology in Practice . 24 June 2022 . Aeronautical Decision Making: The next generation. 2017 . 223–254 . 10.4324/9781351218825-11 . 9781351218825 .
  7. 2023. 2. 7-8.
  8. Web site: Human Factor and Pilot Decision-making. 2015. Parry. David.
  9. Web site: Setting Priorities - Aviate, Navigate, Communicate . iflyamerica.org . 20 April 2023.
  10. Web site: Flight-crew human factors handbook CAP 737 . UK CAA . 23 June 2022.
  11. Web site: Faraz . DODAR - A breakdown for Aviators . @FlightCopilot . 23 June 2022 . en . 11 December 2017.
  12. Web site: DODAR - IVAO - International Virtual Aviation Organisation . mediawiki.ivao.aero . 23 June 2022.
  13. Web site: CRM tools . pmFlight Training . 23 June 2022.
  14. Li . Wen-Chin . Li . Lun-Wen . Harris . Don . Hsu . Yueh-Ling . The Application of Aeronautical Decision-making Support Systems for Improving Pilots' Performance in Flight Operations . Journal of Aeronautics, Astronautics and Aviation . 1 June 2014 . 46 . 2 . 10.6125/14-0324-789 . 1 August 2022.
  15. Web site: FOR-DEC . SKYbrary Aviation Safety . 23 June 2022 . en . 27 May 2021.
  16. Book: Martinussen . Monica . Hunter . David R. . Aviation Psychology and Human Factors . 12 July 2017 . CRC Press . 978-1-351-64901-8 . 35 . 23 June 2022 . en.
  17. Murray . Stephen R. . Deliberate Decision Making by Aircraft Pilots: A Simple Reminder to Avoid Decision Making Under Panic . The International Journal of Aviation Psychology . 1 January 1997 . 7 . 1 . 83–100 . 10.1207/s15327108ijap0701_5 . 1 August 2022 . en . 1050-8414.
  18. Li . Wen-Chin . Harris . Don . Aeronautical decision making: instructor-pilot evaluation of five mnemonic methods . Aviation, Space, and Environmental Medicine . December 2005 . 76 . 12 . 1156–1161 . 16370266 . 1 August 2022.
  19. Book: MacLeod . Norman . Crew Resource Management Training: A Competence-based Approach for Airline Pilots . 5 May 2021 . CRC Press . 978-1-000-37668-5 . 27 July 2022 . en.
  20. 2023. 2. 15.
  21. Book: Smejkal . Petr . The Command Handbook: A Practical Guide through Command Upgrade and Beyond . 737 Publishing s.r.o. . 40 . 27 July 2022 . en.
  22. Web site: How do Pilots Make Decisions? . FlightDeckFriend.com . 23 June 2022.
  23. Soll . Henning . Proske . Solveig . Hofinger . Gesine . Steinhardt . Gunnar . Decision-Making Tools for Aeronautical Teams: FOR-DEC and Beyond . Aviation Psychology and Applied Human Factors . 1 September 2016 . 6 . 2 . 101–112 . 10.1027/2192-0923/a000099 . 23 June 2022.
  24. Book: Roth . Wolff-Michael . Cognition, Assessment and Debriefing in Aviation . 20 July 2017 . CRC Press . 978-1-351-80702-9 . 1 August 2022 . en.
  25. Chirp Cabin Crew Feedback . CHIRP Confidential Reporting Programme . March 2013 . 48 . 23 June 2022.
  26. Web site: What is a NITS Briefing - What is the NITS Briefing? - BizJet Business Jet Flight Safety Equipment Fire Fighting Emergency Evacuation . bizjetsafetyequipment.com . 23 June 2022 . en.
  27. Book: Caldwell, John. Travel Medicine and Infectious Disease. Elsevier. 2004. 85–96.
  28. National Transportation Safety Board. 2000. Controlled Flight Into Terrain, Korean Air Flight 801, Boeing 747-300, HL7468, Nimitz Hill, Guam, August 6, 1997. Aircraft Accident Report NTSB/AAR-00/01. Washington, DC.
  29. Aviation accident report. 2006. Collision with Trees and Crash Short of Runway, Corporate Airlines Flight 5966.
  30. loss of control on Approach Colgan Air. National transportation safety board. February 12, 2009. Accident Report.
  31. Accident to Air India Express at Mangalore. October 31, 2010. https://web.archive.org/web/20180516231449/http://dgca.nic.in/accident/reports/VT-AXV.pdf. May 16, 2018. dead.
  32. Stress and job satisfaction among air force military pilots. Tehran. Iran. 2007.
  33. Web site: Aeronautical Decision-Making. 2013. FAA . dead . https://web.archive.org/web/20151208062625/https://www.faa.gov/regulations_policies/handbooks_manuals/aviation/pilot_handbook/media/PHAK%20-%20Chapter%2017.pdf . 2015-12-08.
  34. Book: SKITKA, Linda. Does automation bias decision-making?. Elsevier. 1999.
  35. Error, stress, and teamwork in medicine and aviation: cross sectional surveys. BMJ. 2000-03-18. 0959-8138. 10720356. 745–749. 320. 7237. 10.1136/bmj.320.7237.745. J. Bryan. Sexton. Eric J.. Thomas. Robert L.. Helmreich. 27316.
  36. 'Pressing On' Into Deteriorating Conditions: An Application of Behavioral Decision Theory to Pilot Decision Making. O'Hare. David. 13 Nov 2009. Aviation Psychology. 10.1207/s15327108ijap0504_2. 5. 4. 351–370.
  37. Download Limit Exceeded. 10.1.1.434.3878.
  38. Web site: Expertise in aeronautical weather-related decision making: A cross-sectional analysis of general aviation pilots.. APA PsycNET. 2015-10-31.
  39. Does automation bias decision-making?. International Journal of Human-Computer Studies. 1999-11-01. 991–1006. 51. 5. 10.1006/ijhc.1999.0252. LINDA J.. SKITKA. KATHLEEN L.. MOSIER. MARK. BURDICK. 1863226.
  40. Emergency decision making. Irving. Janis. 1977. Journal of Human Stress. 10.1080/0097840X.1977.9936085. 864252. 3. 2. 35–48.