Wahgi | |
States: | Papua New Guinea |
Region: | Western Highlands Province |
Speakers: | 86,000 |
Date: | 1999 |
Ref: | e18 |
Familycolor: | Papuan |
Fam1: | Trans–New Guinea |
Fam2: | Chimbu–Wahgi |
Fam3: | Wahgi languages |
Lc1: | wgi |
Ld1: | Mid-Wahgi |
Lc2: | whg |
Ld2: | North Wahgi |
Glotto: | nucl1620 |
Glottoname: | Nuclear Wahgi |
Glottorefname: | Wahgi |
Glotto2: | nort2921 |
Glottoname2: | North Wahgi |
Wahgi is a Trans–New Guinea language of the Chimbu–Wahgi branch spoken by approximately 100,000 people in the highlands of Papua New Guinea. Like other Chimbu languages, Wahgi has some unusual lateral consonants.
Bilabial | (palatalized) dental[2] | Alveolar | Palatal | Velar | ||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Nasal | pronounced as /ink/ | pronounced as /ink/~pronounced as /ink/ | pronounced as /ink/ | pronounced as /ink/ | ||||
Obstruent | pronounced as /ink/ | pronounced as /ink/~pronounced as /ink/ | pronounced as /ink/ | pronounced as /ink/ | ||||
pronounced as /ink/ | pronounced as /ink/~pronounced as /ink/ | pronounced as /ink/ | pronounced as /ink/ | |||||
Lateral | pronounced as /ink/~pronounced as /ink/ | pronounced as /ink/ | pronounced as /ink/ | |||||
Semivowel | pronounced as /ink/ | pronounced as /ink/ |
The dental consonants are "distributed", with closure along a considerable distance of the vocal tract. This presumably means that they are laminal, and that the alveolar consonants are apical. The dental consonants have palatalized allophones in free variation.
The description of the (palatalized) dentals suggest they may be alveolo-palatal or something similar. This is further suggested by transitional vowels triggered by pronounced as //ɬ̪//, apparently even by its non-palatalized allophone: pronounced as //oɬ̪/ [oɪ̯ɬ̪]/ 'moon', pronounced as //ˈéɬ̪é/ [ˈéɬ̪ɪ̯è]/ [sic] 'here'.
The nasal element of the prenasalized consonants is syllabic when not preceded by a vowel, but takes a noncontrastive low tone and is never stressed: pronounced as //ˈmbà/ [m̩̀ˈba]/ 'but', pronounced as //ˈpɪ̀ɬ̪mbé/ [ˈpɪ̀ɬ̪m̩̄bé]/ 'we know'. Prenasalized consonants are perceived as single segments word-initially, but as nasal + stop word-medially.[3]
Nasals and semivowels have no notable allophony, apart from the optional palatalization of pronounced as //n̪//. The other consonants vary markedly, with most variants being positional:
word-initial | word-medial | word-final | word-initial, phrase-medial | word-final, phrase-medial | |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
/p/ | p~b pʷ~bʷ[4] | p~b | pʰ~ɸ | pʰ~ɸ~pᵊ[5] | |
/mb/ | ᵐp~ᵐb | mp~mb | mpʰ~mɸ | mpʰ~mɸ~mpᵊ | |
/s/ | t̪~t̪s̪~s̪~t̪ɕ~ɕ s̪ʷ | t̪s̪~s̪~t̪ɕ~ɕ t̪[6] | t̪s̪~s̪~t̪ɕ~ɕ | [7] | |
/nz/ | ⁿd̪~ⁿd̪z̪~ⁿd̪ʑ~ⁿt̪~ⁿt̪s̪~ⁿt̪ɕ | n̪d̪~n̪d̪z̪~n̪d̪ʑ~n̪t̪~n̪t̪s̪~n̪t̪ɕ | n̪t̪~n̪t̪s̪~n̪t̪ɕ~n̪s̪~n̪ɕ | ||
/t/ | t~d tʷ | ɾ~r | r̥ | t~d[8] ɾ~r[9] | ɾ~r~r̥ t̚[10] |
/nd/ | ⁿt~ⁿd | nt~nd | ntʰ | ntʰ~ntᵊ | |
/k/ | k~ɡ kʷ~ɡʷ[11] | k~ɡ ŋ (?)[12] | kʰ | ||
/ng/ | ᵑk~ᵑɡ ᵑɡʷ | ŋk~ŋɡ | (does not occur) | ||
/ɬ̪/ | (do not occur) | ɬ̪ˢ̪[13] ~ɬ̪~ɮ̪ | ɬ̪ˢ̪~ɬ̪ | ||
/ɺ/ | ɺ̥~ɺ̥~ɺ̥tʰ~ɺ̥tr̥~ɺ̥r̥~ɺ~ɺ̥d | ɺ̥~ɺ̥~ɺ̥tʰ~ɺ̥tr̥~ɺ̥r̥ | |||
/ʟ/ | ʟ~kʟ~ʟ̝~ɢ̆[14] | ʟ~kʟ |
The southern Kuma dialect has these three laterals, though the alveolar is rather uncommon. (The northern Danga dialect has two laterals, dental fricative and alveolar flap.) The dental and velar laterals assimilate to pronounced as /[ɬ]/ or pronounced as /[ɬ̪]/ before dental and alveolar consonants; the alveolar only to pronounced as /[ɬ]/ before alveolar consonants. In locational forms with a suffix beginning with /t/, the /t/ is elided following the lateral, so that on the surface Wahgi distinguishes dental vs alveolar lateral fricatives and alveolar lateral fricatives vs flaps.
Front | Central | Back | ||
---|---|---|---|---|
Close | pronounced as /link/ | pronounced as /link/ | ||
Near-close | pronounced as /link/ | |||
Mid | pronounced as /link/ | pronounced as /link/ | ||
Open | pronounced as /link/ |
pronounced as //a// appears as pronounced as /[ä]/ in all positions except before /k/ and /p/, where it is pronounced as /[ɐ]/. In medial position it is in occasional free variation with pronounced as /[æ]/.
pronounced as //ɔ// appears as pronounced as /[ɔ]/ in all positions except before nasals, where it is pronounced as /[ɒ]/. Word-finally it is in free variation with pronounced as /[o]/.
pronounced as //u// is reduced to labialization (pronounced as /[ʷ]/) between pronounced as //p t s k ng// and stressed pronounced as //i// or pronounced as //ɪ//.
All vowels neutralize to pronounced as /[ɪ̈]/ in final unstressed closed syllables (disregarding subsequent "clitics").
In rapid speech, unstressed pronounced as //u, ɪ, e// elide between stressed syllables. For example, in rapid speech pronounced as //ˈpetepˈka// is pronounced pronounced as /[ˈpeɾpˈka]/.
Wahgi appears to have three word tones rather than phonemic tone on each syllable. At least one syllable will have a phonetic high tone, with the resulting patterns of high, rising and falling on words of one or two syllables, and peaking, rising and falling on words of three to five syllables. Stress also appears to be contrastive, but analysis is difficult.
On both monosyllabic and disyllabic words, the three tones are realized as high, rising and falling: [HH], [LH] and [HL]. An analysis of two phonemic syllable tones, HIGH and LOW, is complicated by the appearance of all three word tones on monosyllables with final occlusive codas (and phonetically short vowels).[15]
In polysyllabic words,
On trisyllables, the patterns LHL, HLL, and LLH are attested. On tetrasyllables are LLHL, LLLH, and LHHL (that is, LMHL, LLMH, LHHL). On pentasyllables, only two patterns have been found, LLHHH and LLHHL (that is, LMHHH and LMHHL).
Words with six to nine syllables are only attested with a single pattern, a peaking tone (LHL) on the first three syllables followed by a falling tone (HL) stretched out over the remaining syllables. For words of six syllables, this is realized as LHLHLL (phonetically LHLHML); for seven, LHLHLLL; for eight, LHLHHLLL; for nine LHLHHLLLL. In each case, the second syllable (the first high tone) is also stressed, and it is not clear if these are actually double-peaking tones or single peaking tones with preceding stress. Regardless, there does not appear to be any phonemic distinction.
The maximal syllable in Wahgi is CVCC; the minimal syllable is V, which may be any vowel but pronounced as //ɪ//. Any consonant may occur in the onset except the three laterals. Any consonant may occur in the coda except the semivowels and /ng/. With a coda consonant cluster, the first consonant may only be pronounced as //p, t, k, l̪, ʟ// (the two common laterals) and the second may only be pronounced as //m, s//.
A word may be anywhere from one to nine syllables long. A monosyllabic word may be any type of syllable but V and maybe CV. (Phonetic length and perhaps tone distribution suggests that words transcribed as CV monosyllables may actually be CVV with identical vowels.) Beside the syllable-onset and -coda restrictions, pronounced as //i, ɪ// may not occur word-initially and pronounced as //ɪ// may not occur word-finally.
Observed vowel clusters within words are pronounced as //i.a, i.o, i.u; ɪ.i, ɪ.o; e.i, e.o, e.u; a.i, a.ɪ, a.e, a.a, a.u; o.i, o.o; u.i, u.ɪ, u.a, u.o, u.u//. In some cases (such as pronounced as //ɪ.i//) these sequences reduce across morpheme boundaries, and stress seems to play a role in vowel reduction. It may be that some of the difficulties in analyzing stress may be a conflation of vowel sequences across syllables with sequences in single heavy (bimoraic CVV) syllables.
Observed consonant clusters within words are those allowed as syllable codas, pronounced as /link/, plus pronounced as //p.p, p.t, p.nz; mb.p, mb.t, mb.m; t.p, t.mb, t.nd, t.n̪, t.n, t.ŋ; nd.p, nd.m; k.p; s.p, s.mb, s.nd, s.k, s.nz, s.m, s.n; nz.p, nz.m; m.p, m.nz, m.m, m.ŋ; n̪.p, n̪.m; n.p, n.m, n.ŋ; ŋ.p, ŋ.mb, ŋ.s, ŋ.nz, ŋ.m, ŋ.n; l̪.p, l̪.mb, l̪.t, l̪.k, l̪ [sic], l̪.w; l.mb, l.nd; ʟ.p, ʟ.mb, ʟ.t, ʟ.nz, ʟ.n, ʟ.ŋ, ʟ.w//.
In rapid speech, the following additional combinations are known: pronounced as //ks.mp, ks.n, ks.m, ps.nd, lm.ŋ, tm.ŋ//.
Below are some reflexes of proto-Trans-New Guinea proposed by Pawley (2012), drawn from Ramsey (1975):[16]
Middle Wahgi | ||
---|---|---|
| ma ‘no!’ | |
| maɫ | |
| muŋ ‘fruit, nut, lump’ | |
| mokum, mokem ‘knuckle, joint’ | |
| mund-muŋ | |
| mi | |
| mek (si-) ‘vomit’, mek ‘vomitus’ | |
| ama | |
| am | |
| numan | |
| numan ‘thought, mind, will’ | |
| kumb- ‘(of fire) die’ | |
| mokum, mokem, (angeɫ) mokem ‘knuckle, joint’ | |
| no- | |
| numan | |
| numan ‘thought, mind, will’ | |
| (?) mundun mo- ‘be pot-bellied’ | |
| numan | |
| numan | |
| mund-muŋ | |
| ŋaŋ ‘small male child’ | |
| (?) amb ‘woman’, ambi- ‘wife’ | |
| embe(m) | |
| pu | |
| apa- ‘maternal uncle’ | |
| mund-mung | |
| tuk- ‘chop’ | |
| mungum ‘kidney’ | |
| muŋ ‘fruit, nut, lump’ | |
| mund-muŋ | |
| (?) kau- ‘carry on head or shoulder’ | |
| tuk- ‘chop’ | |
| muk | |
| mek (si-), mek ‘vomitus’ | |
| maɫ ‘ground, soil, world’ | |
| noɫ | |
| ŋaɫ ‘small baby’, ŋaŋa ‘male child’ | |
| -iɫ |
To support his proto-Trans-New Guinea reconstructions, Pawley (2012) also cites probable reflexes in the Apali, Kalam, Kâte, Selepet, Binandere, Katei, Kiwai, Telefol, and Asmat languages.
Middle Wahgi distinguishes 14 color terms (from Evelyn Ramsey 1975):[17]
Middle Wahgi has three types of contrasting color patterns.[17]
Middle Wahgi has at least a dozen words for days before or after, going far beyond ‘yesterday’ or ‘tomorrow’ (from Evelyn Ramsey 1975):[17]
The first dictionary of Middle Wahgi was published by Church of the Nazarene medical missionary Evelyn Ramsey in 1975.[18] [19]