Na-Dene | |
Also Known As: | Athabaskan–Eyak–Tlingit |
Map: | Na-Dene languages.svg |
Mapsize: | 250 |
Region: | North America |
Familycolor: | Na-Dene |
Family: | One of the world's primary language families |
Child1: | Tlingit |
Child2: | Athabaskan–Eyak |
Iso5: | xnd |
Glotto: | atha1245 |
Glottorefname: | Athabaskan–Eyak–Tlingit |
Na-Dene (; also Nadene, Na-Dené, Athabaskan–Eyak–Tlingit, Tlina–Dene) is a family of Native American languages that includes at least the Athabaskan languages, Eyak, and Tlingit languages. Haida was formerly included but is now considered doubtful. By far the most widely spoken Na-Dene language today is Navajo, also the most spoken indigenous language north of Mexico.
In February 2008, a proposal connecting Na-Dene (excluding Haida) to the Yeniseian languages of central Siberia into a Dené–Yeniseian family was published and well received by a number of linguists.[1] It was proposed in a 2014 paper that the Na-Dene languages of North America and the Yeniseian languages of Siberia had a common origin in a language spoken in Beringia, between the two continents.[2]
Edward Sapir originally constructed the term Na-Dene to refer to a combined family of Athabaskan, Tlingit, and Haida (the existence of the Eyak language was not known to him at the time). In his “The Na-Dene languages: A preliminary report”, he describes how he arrived at the term (Sapir 1915, p. 558):
In its uncontroversial core, Na-Dene consists of two branches, Tlingit and Athabaskan–Eyak:
For linguists who follow Edward Sapir in connecting Haida to the above languages, Haida represents an additional branch, with Athabaskan–Eyak–Tlingit together forming the other. Dene or Dine (the Athabaskan languages) is a widely distributed group of Native languages spoken by associated peoples in Alberta, British Columbia, Manitoba, Northwest Territories, Nunavut, Saskatchewan, Yukon, Alaska, parts of Oregon, northern California, and the American Southwest as far as northern Mexico.
The southwestern division of Athabaskan is also called Southern Athabaskan or Apachean, and includes Navajo and all the Apache languages. Eyak was spoken in south-central Alaska; the last first language speaker died in 2008. Navajo is by far the most widely spoken language of the Na-Dene family, spoken in Arizona, New Mexico, and other regions of the American Southwest.
All of these languages share a highly complex prefixing verb structure in which tense and mood markers are interdigitated between subject and object agreement markers. The morphological hallmark of the family is a series of prefixes found directly before the verb root that raise or lower the transitivity of the verb word. These prefixes, traditionally known as "classifiers", derive historically from a combination of three distinct classes of morphemes and are not found in any other Native American language family.
The phoneme system contains a large number of dorsal (velar or uvular) consonants (fronting in many modern Athabaskan languages to palatals and velars, correspondingly) as well as a general absence of labial obstruents (except where /b/ has arisen from *w). In the historical phonology there is a widespread tendency, observable across many Athabaskan languages, for phonemic tonal distinctions to arise from glottal features originally found at the end of the syllable. The glottal features in question are often evident in Eyak or Tlingit. These languages are typologically unusual in containing extensive prefixation yet being SOV and postpositional, features normally associated with suffixing languages.
A genealogical connection between the Tlingit, Eyak and Athabaskan languages was suggested early in the 19th century, but not universally accepted until much later. Haida, with 15 fluent speakers (M. Krauss, 1995), was originally linked to Tlingit by Franz Boas in 1894. Both Haida and Tlingit were then connected to Athabaskan by Edward Sapir in 1915. Linguists such as Lyle Campbell (1997) today consider the evidence inconclusive. They have classified Haida as a language isolate. In order to emphasise the exclusion of Haida, Campbell refers to the language family as Athabaskan–Eyak–Tlingit rather than Na-Dene. In 2010 Jeff Leer published extensive primary materials on what he calls PAET (Proto-Athabaskan–Eyak–Tlingit).
See main article: Dené–Yeniseian languages.
In 2008, Edward Vajda of Western Washington University presented evidence suggesting that the Na-Dene languages (Athabaskan–Eyak–Tlingit) might be related to the Yeniseian (or Yeniseic) languages of Siberia,[4] the only living representative of which is the Ket language.
Key evidence by current comparative methodologies includes homologies in verb prefixes and also a systematic correspondence between the distribution of Ket tones and consonant articulations found in Athabaskan–Eyak–Tlingit. Vajda's paper has been favorably reviewed by several experts on Na-Dene and Yeniseic languages, including Michael Krauss, Jeff Leer, James Kari, and Heinrich Werner, as well as a number of other well-known linguists, including Bernard Comrie, Johanna Nichols, Victor Golla, Michael Fortescue, and Eric Hamp. The conclusion of this seminar was that the comparison with Yeniseic data shows that Haida cannot be classified in a genealogical unit with Athabaskan–Eyak–Tlingit.[1]
See main article: Sino-Tibetan languages.
A link between the Na–Dené languages and Sino-Tibetan languages, known as Sino–Dené was proposed by Edward Sapir. Around 1920 Sapir became convinced that Na-Dené was more closely related to Sino-Tibetan than to other American families.[5] He wrote a series of letters to Alfred Kroeber where he enthusiastically spoke of a connection between Na-Dene and "Indo-Chinese". In 1925, a supporting article summarizing his thoughts, albeit not written by him, entitled "The Similarities of Chinese and Indian Languages", was published in Science Supplements.[6]
Edward Vadja's Dené–Yeniseian proposal renewed interest among linguists such as Geoffrey Caveney (2014) to look into support for the Sino–Dené hypothesis. Caveney considered a link between Sino-Tibetan, Na-Dené, and Yeniseian to be plausible but did not support the hypothesis that Sino-Tibetan and Na-Dené were related to the Caucasian languages (Sino–Caucasian and Dené–Caucasian).[7]
A 2023 analysis by David Bradley using the standard techniques of comparative linguistics supports a distant genetic link between the Sino-Tibetan, Na-Dené, and Yeniseian language families. Bradley argues that any similarities Sino-Tibetan shares with other language families of the East Asia area such as Hmong-Mien, Altaic (which is actually a sprachbund), Austroasiatic, Kra-Dai, Austronesian came through contact; but as there has been no recent contact between Sino-Tibetan, Na-Dené, and Yeniseian language families then any similarities these groups share must be residual.[8]
According to Joseph Greenberg's controversial classification of the languages of Native North America, Na-Dené (including Haida) is one of the three main groups of Native languages spoken in the Americas. Contemporary supporters of Greenberg's theory, such as Merritt Ruhlen, have suggested that the Na-Dené language family represents a distinct migration of people from Asia into the New World that occurred six to eight thousand years ago, placing it around four thousand years later than the previous migration into the Americas by Amerind speakers; this remains an unproven hypothesis.[9] Ruhlen speculates that the Na-Dené speakers may have arrived in boats, initially settling near the Haida Gwaii, now in British Columbia, Canada.[10]
Bouda, in various publications in the 1930s through the 1950s, described a linguistic network that (besides Yeniseian and Sino-Tibetan) also included Caucasian, and Burushaski, some forms of which have gone by the name of Sino-Caucasian. The works of R. Bleichsteiner[11] and O.G. Tailleur,[12] the late Sergei A. Starostin[13] and Sergei L. Nikolayev[14] have sought to confirm these connections. Others who have developed the hypothesis, often expanded to Dené–Caucasian, include J.D. Bengtson,[15] V. Blažek,[16] J.H. Greenberg (with M. Ruhlen),[17] and M. Ruhlen.[18] George Starostin continues his father's work in Yeniseian, Sino-Caucasian and other fields.[19] This theory is very controversial or viewed as obsolete by other linguists.[20] [21] [22]
Speakers of the Na-Dene languages, while mostly closely related to other North American indigenous peoples, derive around 10% of their ancestry from a Siberian source closely related to Koryaks not found in other Native American groups. The contact between the ancestors of Na-Dene speakers and this Siberian group is suggested to have occurred around 9,000-5,500 years ago.[23] The urheimat (origin point of the family) has been suggested to have been in Alaska.[24] A large southward migration of Athabaskan peoples is thought to have occurred around 1,000 years ago, resulting in the settlement of southern North America.[25]
This phonological chart shows where the listed varieties have sounds which are the same, similar, and sometimes different. The sounds shown, obstruents, are a particular class of consonants. Where similarities are found between one or more varieties, this presents at least some evidence of genetic relatedness among those varieties.
Obstruent correspondences | ||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
PAET | PAE | PA | Eyak | Tlingit | ||
Normal | L-assim. | |||||
pronounced as /d/ | ||||||
pronounced as /t/ | ||||||
pronounced as /tʼ/ | ||||||
pronounced as /tɬʼ/ | pronounced as /ɬʼ/, pronounced as /tɬʼ/ | |||||
pronounced as /ɬ/ | pronounced as /ɬ~l/ | pronounced as /ɬ/ | ||||
pronounced as /dʒ/ | ? | |||||
pronounced as /tʃ/ | pronounced as /tʃ/ (pronounced as /ts/) | pronounced as /tɬ/ | ||||
pronounced as /tʃʼ/ | pronounced as /sʼ/, pronounced as /tʃʼ/ (pronounced as /tsʼ/) | pronounced as /tɬʼ/ | ||||
pronounced as /ʃ/ | pronounced as /ʂ~ʐ/ | pronounced as /ʃ/ (pronounced as /s/) | pronounced as /ɬ/ | |||
pronounced as /ɡʲ/ | pronounced as /dz/ | pronounced as /[dz]/, pronounced as /s~z/ | pronounced as /dz/ | pronounced as /ɡ/ | ||
pronounced as /ts/ | pronounced as /ts/ | pronounced as /ts/ | pronounced as /tɬ/ | |||
pronounced as /kʲ/ | pronounced as /k/, pronounced as /ʃ/ | |||||
pronounced as /tsʼ/ | pronounced as /tsʼ/ | pronounced as /sʼ/, pronounced as /tsʼ/ | pronounced as /tɬʼ/, pronounced as /ɬ/ | |||
pronounced as /kʲʼ/ | pronounced as /kʼ/ | |||||
pronounced as /s/ | pronounced as /s/ | pronounced as /s~z/ | pronounced as /s/ | pronounced as /ʃ/ | ||
pronounced as /xʲ/ | pronounced as /s/
| pronounced as /x/ | ||||
pronounced as /ɡ/ | pronounced as /ɡʲ/ | pronounced as /ɡ/ | pronounced as /ɡ(ʷ)/ | |||
pronounced as /ɡʷ/ | pronounced as /ɖʐ/ | pronounced as /ɡʷ → ɡ/ | ||||
pronounced as /k/ | pronounced as /kʲ/ | pronounced as /k/ | pronounced as /k(ʷ)/ | |||
pronounced as /kʷ/ | pronounced as /ʈʂ/ | pronounced as /kʷ → k/ | ||||
pronounced as /kʼ/ | pronounced as /kʲʼ/ | pronounced as /kʼ/ | pronounced as /xʼ(ʷ)/, pronounced as /kʼ(ʷ)/ | |||
pronounced as /kʷʼ/ | pronounced as /ʈʂʼ/ | pronounced as /kʼʷ → kʼ/ | ||||
pronounced as /x/ | pronounced as /xʲ~j/ | pronounced as /x/ | pronounced as /x/ | |||
pronounced as /xʷ/ | pronounced as /ʂ~ʐ/ | pronounced as /xʷ → x/ | ||||
pronounced as /ɢ/ | pronounced as /ɢ/ | pronounced as /ɢ(ʷ)/ | ||||
pronounced as /ɢʷ/ | pronounced as /ɢʷ → ɢ/ | |||||
pronounced as /q/ | pronounced as /q/ | pronounced as /q(ʷ)/ | ||||
pronounced as /qʷ/ | pronounced as /qʷ → q/ | |||||
pronounced as /qʼ/ | pronounced as /qʼ/ | pronounced as /χʼ(ʷ)/ | ||||
pronounced as /qʷʼ/ | pronounced as /qʷʼ → qʼ/ | pronounced as /χʼ, qʼ(ʷ)/ | ||||
pronounced as /χ/ | pronounced as /χ~ʁ/ | pronounced as /χ/ | pronounced as /χ(ʷ)/ | |||
pronounced as /χʷ/ | pronounced as /χʷ → χ~ʁ/ | pronounced as /χʷ/ | ||||
Extrasystematic fricative correspondences | ||||||
pronounced as /sx/ | pronounced as /x/ | pronounced as /xʲ~j/ | pronounced as /x/ | pronounced as /s/ | ||
pronounced as /ʃx/ | pronounced as /ʃ/ | |||||
$ | pronounced as /x(ʷ) ?/ | $ (pronounced as /ʃ~xʲ/) | pronounced as /xʷ → x/
| pronounced as /χ/ |
Table notes: