Kara language (Papua New Guinea) explained

Kara
Region:New Ireland Province, Papua New Guinea
Speakers:5,000
Date:1998
Ref:e18
Familycolor:Austronesian
Fam2:Malayo-Polynesian
Fam3:Oceanic
Fam4:Western
Fam5:Meso-Melanesian
Fam6:(New Ireland)
Fam7:Tungag–Nalik
Script:Latin
Iso3:leu
Glotto:kara1486
Glottorefname:Kara (Papua New Guinea)

Kara (also Lemusmus or Lemakot) is an Austronesian language spoken by about 5,000 people in 1998 in the Kavieng District of New Ireland Province, Papua New Guinea.

Laxudumau, spoken in the village of Lakudumau, is transitional to Nalik.

Phonology

Consonants

Consonant phonemes!!Labial!Alveolar!Velar!Uvular
Nasalpronounced as /ink/pronounced as /ink/pronounced as /ink/
Plosivepronounced as /ink/ pronounced as /ink/pronounced as /ink/ pronounced as /ink/pronounced as /ink/pronounced as /ink/
Fricativepronounced as /ink/ pronounced as /ink/pronounced as /ink/pronounced as /ink/
Trillpronounced as /ink/
Lateralpronounced as /ink/

Kara contains fourteen consonants. Single consonants are found within the head of a word, intervocalically between two vowels, finally and in sequences of less than two words medially. Voiceless consonants pronounced as //p, t, q, ɸ, s// create a cluster on the second consonant. Voiced consonants pronounced as //b, d, g, β, ɣ// appear initially and intervocally. They appear as the second consonant of a cluster. An example would be pronounced as /[βalβal]/ 'tree sap'.[1] It is notable that different dialects change the use of consonants. West Kara replaces pronounced as //s// with pronounced as //z// anytime it would proceed a vowel, and interpolate pronounced as //ɸ// with pronounced as /[h]/ before a vowel and pronounced as /[ʔ]/ at the end of a word.

Examples of Consonants Used!Consonant!Head (Initial)!Intervocalic!Final
ppronounced as /[pʰabʊŋ]/ 'clan'pronounced as /[ipʰʊl]/ 'surprise'pronounced as /[lɛp]/ 'wave'
tpronounced as /[tʰuɸ]/ 'sugar cane'pronounced as /[xutʰat]/ 'crayfish'pronounced as /[ɸat]/ 'stone'
qpronounced as /[qʰɔɾ]/ 'raven'pronounced as /[xɔqʰɔɸ]/ 'head cloth'pronounced as /[laq]/ 'go up'
gpronounced as /[gis]/ 'sick'pronounced as /[gogon]/ 'sweep'does not occur

Vowels

Vowel phones!!Front!Central!Back
Closepronounced as /ink/pronounced as /ink/
Near-Closepronounced as /ink/pronounced as /ink/
Close-Midpronounced as /ink/pronounced as /ink/
Open-Midpronounced as /ink/pronounced as /ink/pronounced as /ink/
Openpronounced as /ink/

Kara contains ten vowels. Relative to their position in the IPA vowel chart, the vowels in Kara tend to contrast each other throughout the language. Central vowels pronounced as /[a]/ and pronounced as /[ə]/ contrast in both open and closed syllables.[2] Example:

Mid vowels pronounced as /[e]/ and pronounced as /[ɛ]/, and pronounced as /[o]/ and pronounced as /[ɔ]/ are complementary to each other in their respective pairs. Each pair occurs in open syllables, a syllable consisting of an onset and nucleus but no coda.

Higher vowels pronounced as /[i]/ and pronounced as /[ɪ]/, and pronounced as /[u]/ and pronounced as /[ʊ]/ contrast in closed syllables, a syllable consisting of an onset, nucleus, and coda.

Stress

Kara has an unusual occurrence of stress, or relative emphasis of syllables. Stress in Kara occurs on any syllable in a word, but follows a system of rules that allow placement of stress in an ordered system in all words that contain two syllables or more. However, syllables stemmed from prefixes are never stressed regardless of the ordered system. Stress is determined by three factors: vowel quality, syllable closure, and position in the word, with vowel quality being the most important factor.[3]

Syllables and stress

A syllable with a nucleus of pronounced as //a// receives primary stress regardless of its position in the word.

A word with more than one syllable and a nucleus of pronounced as //a// has the stress fall on the last syllable..

A word with neither a syllable with a nucleus of pronounced as //a// or a closed syllable has the stress fall on the initial syllable.

Grammar

Kara follows a verb–subject–object word order, and uses reduplication for creation of more complex sentences. The language determines that body parts and kinship terms must be identified as belonging to someone which make it possible for inalienable possession.[4]

Verbs

Verbs in Kara are used transitively, meaning that the verbs in Kara span a spectrum that ultimately determines how speaking the language occurs between an object and that verb. Transitivity of verbs is used to also determine whether or not the spoken language is between two people actively who are actively speakers, rather than a speaker and a listener. For example: the verb pronounced as /[kuus]/ 'say' has an initiator but no one to actively speak to.[5]

Reduplication

Reduplication occurs in words with an initial plosive consonant. The reduplicated consonant created is said fricatively within the same articulation of the word.

Inalienable possession

Inalienable suffixes!Stem!1s poss!2s poss!3s poss!Meaning
pronounced as /[nasə]/pronounced as /[nasaq]/pronounced as /[nasam]/pronounced as /[nasənə]/'wife'
pronounced as /[mɘtʰɘ]/pronounced as /[mɘtʰaq]/pronounced as /[mɘtʰam]/pronounced as /[mətʰɘnɘ]/'eye'
pronounced as /[βəsa]/pronounced as /[βɘsaq]/pronounced as /[βɘsam]/pronounced as /[βəsanə]/'sibling'
pronounced as /[mi]/pronounced as /[mieq]/pronounced as /[mim]/pronounced as /[minə]/'back'
pronounced as /[ɤu]/pronounced as /[ɤuəq]/pronounced as /[ɤum]/pronounced as /[ɤunɘ]/'stomach'
Note that most of the second-person forms do not contain pronounced as /[ə]/; this occurs because sequences such as pronounced as //iɘ, uɘ, oɘ, eə// only occur before pronounced as //ɤ// or pronounced as //q//. Since pronounced as //ɘ// is the second vowel in a sequence, pronounced as //ə// is either combined or deleted before any consonant besides pronounced as //ɤ// and pronounced as //q//.

Bibliography

Notes and References

  1. Schlie, Perry, & Schlie, Ginny. (n.d.). A Kara Phonology. In Phonologies of Austronesian Languages, II (Data Papers in Papua New Guinea Languages, pp. 100). Ukarumpa via Lae: Summer Inst. of Ling.
  2. Schlie, Perry, & Schlie, Ginny. (n.d.). A Kara Phonology. In Phonologies of Austronesian Languages, II (Data Papers in Papua New Guinea Languages, pp. 102). Ukarumpa via Lae: Summer Inst. of Ling.
  3. Schlie, Perry, & Schlie, Ginny. (n.d.). A Kara Phonology. In Phonologies of Austronesian Languages, II (Data Papers in Papua New Guinea Languages, pp. 109). Ukarumpa via Lae: Summer Inst. of Ling.
  4. Schlie, Perry, & Schlie, Ginny. (n.d.). A Kara Phonology. In Phonologies of Austronesian Languages, II (Data Papers in Papua New Guinea Languages, pp. 117). Ukarumpa via Lae: Summer Inst. of Ling.
  5. Franklin, K., & Summer Institute of Linguistics. Papua New Guinea Branch. (1989). Studies in componential analysis (Data papers on Papua New Guinea languages ; vol. no. 36, pg. 39-45). Ukarumpa via Lae, Papua New Guinea: Summer Institute of Linguistics.