Neveʻei | |
Also Known As: | Vinmavis |
Region: | Central Malekula |
State: | Vanuatu |
Speakers: | 500 |
Date: | 2007 |
Ref: | e25 |
Ethnicity: | 710 ethnic population (no date) |
Familycolor: | Austronesian |
Fam2: | Malayo-Polynesian |
Fam3: | Oceanic |
Fam4: | Southern Oceanic |
Fam5: | North-Central Vanuatu |
Fam6: | Central Vanuatu |
Fam7: | Malakula |
Fam8: | Malakula Interior |
Iso3: | vnm |
Glotto: | vinm1237 |
Glottorefname: | Neveʻei |
Map: | Lang Status 99-NE.svg |
Neveʻei (pronounced as /neveʔei/), also known as Vinmavis, is an Oceanic language of central Malekula, Vanuatu.[1] There are around 500 primary speakers of Neveʻei and about 750 speakers in total.
Neveʻei was described for the first time by Jill Musgrave, in a monograph published in 2007: A grammar of Neveʻei, Vanuatu. This book is the source for the data and analyses below.
The traditional name of the language, which is recognized by older speakers, is Neveʻei. However, the majority of younger speakers of Neveʻei do not use the traditional name and some are not even aware of it. Native speakers commonly refer to their language as Nabusian teget which literally means “our language” and in Bislama the language (Neveʻei) is called Lanwis Vinmavis “the language of Vinmavis”. Neveʻei is the traditional name for the language; Vinmavis is the name of one of the villages in which the language is spoken in (Lynch and Crowley 2001:83).[2]
Neveʻei contains 5 vowels and 20 consonants:
Front | Back | ||
---|---|---|---|
High | pronounced as /link/ | pronounced as /link/ | |
Mid | pronounced as /link/ | pronounced as /link/ | |
Low | pronounced as /link/ |
Labio-velar | Labial | Alveolar | Velar | Glottal | |||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Nasal | pronounced as /mʷ/ | pronounced as /link/ | pronounced as /link/ | pronounced as /link/ | |||
Stop | Voiceless | pronounced as /link/ | pronounced as /link/ | pronounced as /link/ | |||
Voiced | pronounced as /bʷ/ | pronounced as /link/ | pronounced as /link/ | pronounced as /link/ | |||
Fricative | pronounced as /vʷ/ | pronounced as /link/ | pronounced as /link/ | pronounced as /link/ | pronounced as /link/ | ||
Lateral | pronounced as /link/ | ||||||
Flap | pronounced as /link/ | ||||||
Glide | pronounced as /link/ | pronounced as /link/ |
By comparison with other languages of the world,[5] such a phoneme inventory classifies Neveʻei in the average range regarding vowels (5-6) as well as regarding consonants (22 ± 3). The consonant-vowel ratio classifies Neveʻei in the low band.
The table above indicates that the segments /t/, /k/ and /ʔ/ can be understood as voiceless stops at the alveolar, velar and glottal positions of articulation (Musgrave, 2007, p. 6). The segment /t/ can be understood as a voiceless alveolar stop in each position: initial, medial and final. For example, (Musgrave, 2007, p. 6):
/tabin/ | [taᵐbɪn] | 'grandfather' | |
/nebat/ | [neᵐbat] | 'vine variety' | |
/atl/ | [atl] | 'third' |
The segment /k/ can be understood as a voiceless velar stop (Musgrave, 2007, p. 7). It rarely appears with non-borrowed lexical root forms and is only found within the root /niaɾekaʔa-n/ 'fin-CONST' (Musgrave, 2007, p. 7).
The segment /ʔ/ can be understood as a glottal stop and appears in all positions, for example (Musgrave, 2007, p. 7):
/ʔaʔan/ | [ʔaʔan] | 'eat' | |
/nibiʔiŋ/ | [nɪᵐbiʔɪŋ] | 'giant turban snail' | |
/bʷebaʔ/ | [ᵐbʷeᵐbaʔ] | 'hide' |
/bʷi-/ | [ᵐbʷi-] | '3SG.IRR (verbal prefix)' | |
/abʷit-/ | [aᵐbʷɪt-] | '2/3PL.IRR (verbal prefix)' | |
/nebʷelegen/ | [neᵐbʷeleᵑgen] | 'thigh' |
/baxah/ | [ᵐbaxah ~ ᵐbaɣah] | 'clean' | |
/nubuah/ | [nuᵐbu(w)ah] | 'pig' |
/doŋon/ | [ⁿdoŋon] | 'count' | |
/dedan/ | [ⁿdeⁿdan] | 'dive, swim underwater' |
/minsed/ | [mɪntʃeⁿd ~ mɪntʃeⁿt ~ mɪntʃeⁿdɾ ~ mɪntʃenɾ] | |
/netarbad/ | [netaɾᵐbaⁿd ~ netaɾᵐbaⁿt ~ netaɾᵐbaⁿdɾ ~ netaɾᵐbanɾ] |
/gis/ | [ᵑgɪs] | 'squash, squeeze' | |
/gegeɾah/ | [ᵑgeᵑgeɾah] | 'drag, pull' |
/mʷaʔam/ | [mʷaʔam] | 'sit' | |
/mamʷe/ | [mamʷe] | 'father' | |
/nemʷen/ | [nemʷen] | 'man' |
/mesemah/ | [mesemah] | 'dry' | |
/nomomox/ | [nomomox] | 'woman' | |
/dam/ | [ⁿdam] | 'shout' |
/nabuŋ/ | [naᵐbuŋ] | 'time, occasion, day' | |
/vanili/ | [fanili] | 'different, strange, unusual' | |
/dan/ | [ⁿdan] | 'go down, sink' |
/ŋaŋ/ | [ŋaŋ] | 'laugh' | |
/doŋon/ | [ⁿdoŋon] | 'count' | |
/naʔaibuŋ/ | [naʔaiᵐbuŋ] | 'my grandchild' |
/vʷelem/ | [fʷelem] | 'come' | |
/nevʷen/ | [nefʷen ~ nevʷen] | 'fruit' |
/vavu/ | [fafu ~ favu] | 'walk' | |
/navmolto/ | [napmolto] | 'kind of bird' | |
/tenev/ | [tenef ~ tenep] | 'yesterday' |
/sido/ | [sɪⁿdo] | 'remember' | |
/bus/ | [ᵐbus] | 'speak, talk' | |
/nsev/ | [ntʃef] | 'cough' |
/xus/ | [xus] | 'hit, kill' | |
/buxut/ | [ᵐbuxut ~ ᵐbuɣut] | 'inside' | |
/maʔabux/ | [maʔaᵐbux] | 'short' |
/vuluh/ | [fuluh] | 'bake in earth oven' | |
/ahau/ | [ahau] | 'no, yes (depending on the question)' | |
/eheʔ/ | [eheʔ] | 'no' |
/lax/ | [lax] | 'hang' | |
/bʷeli/ | [ᵐbʷeli] | 'very, a lot' | |
/namul/ | [namul] | 'million' |
/ɾaʔ/ | [ɾaʔ] | 'work (in garden)' | |
/viviɾoŋ/ | [fifiɾoŋ ~ fiviɾoŋ] | 'listen' | |
/boɾ/ | [ᵐboɾ] | 'deaf' |
/wal/ | [wal] | 'because' | |
/muluwul/ | [muluwul] | 'round' | |
/lowi/ | [lowi] | 'out, away' |
/jaʔai/ | [jaʔai] | 'this person' | |
/joxjox/ | [joxjox] | 'vomit' | |
/nijim/ | [nijɪm] | 'house' |
The consonants that are phonetically similar in Neveʻei can be contrasted in the following ways (Musgrave, 2007, p. 10-11):
/b/ | /beɾ/ | 'long, tall, deep' | |
/bʷ/ | /bʷeɾ/ | 'possibly, maybe' |
/m/ | /melemal/ | 'have cramp' | |
/mʷ/ | /mʷelemal/ | 'straight, correct' |
/v/ | /veɾi/ | 'outside' | |
/vʷ/ | /vʷeɾi/ | 'saying (something), tell (story)' |
/ʔ/ | /meɾaʔ/ | 'get up, fly, jump up' | |
/h/ | /meɾah/ | 'light (in weight)' |
/h/ | /veh/ | 'carry' | |
/∅/ | /ve∅/ | 'what' |
/ʔ/ | /saʔ/ | 'go up' | |
/x/ | /sax/ | 'not' |
/d/ | /madl/ | 'in three days' time' | |
/t/ | /matl/ | 'thick, incorrect, uncircumcised' |
/g/ | /gu/ | 'you (pronoun)' | |
/k/ | /ku-/ | '2SG.IRR (verbal prefix)' |
/ŋ/ | /ŋe/ | 'the (demonstrative)' | |
/g/ | /ge/ | 'the very one' |
/l/ | /nebal/ | 'hawk' | |
/ɾ/ | /nebaɾ/ | 'blind person' | |
/n/ | /neban/ | 'woman's headdress' |
/w/ | /wah/ | 'look' | |
/u/ | /uah/ | 'day after tomorrow' |
Contrasts between plain labials /m/, /v/ and /b/ and labio-velars /mʷ/, /vʷ/ and /bʷ/ are neutralised in circumstances such as before front vowels /i/ and /e/. Examples include (Musgrave, 2007, p. 11):
/nimins/ | 'wooden stirring stick' | |
/nimʷinsi/ | 'star' | |
/meli/ | 'wilt' | |
/mʷelam/ | 'marked, spotted' | |
/visvis/ | 'teach' | |
/vʷisi/ | 'train yam vine' | |
/veh/ | 'carry' | |
/vʷelem/ | 'come' | |
/bʷiaŋ/ | 'all right' | |
/bial/ | 'all over the place, all sorts of things' | |
/bʷeɾ/ | 'maybe' | |
/beɾ/ | 'long, tall' |
When these consonants precede rounded vowels such as /o/ and /u/ there is no contrast and only plain labials, examples include (Musgrave, 2007, p. 11):
/moŋ/ | 'proud' | |
/numuɾ/ | 'person' | |
/vov/ | 'rain' | |
/navusmo/ | 'white flying fox' | |
/boɾ/ | 'deaf' | |
/bubut/ | 'do quietly' |
The table above (Musgrave, 2007, p. 12) indicates that the vowel /i/ can be understood as a tense high front unrounded vowel (Musgrave, 2007, p. 12). When the vowel is followed by a nasal or a prenasalised consonant and in closed syllables that end in a non-liquid alveolar consonant, it can be understood as the lax high front vowel [ɪ] (Musgrave, 2007, p. 12). For example, (Musgrave, 2007, p. 12):
/ivah/ | [ifah ~ ivah] | 'four' | |
/nimin/ | [nɪmɪn] | 'bird' | |
/gis/ | [ᵑgɪs] | 'squash, squeeze' |
/utne/ | [utne] | 'here' | |
/lueh/ | [lu(w)eh] | 'tired' | |
/manuanu/ | [manu(w)anu] | 'rainbow' |
/etnaŋ/ | [etnaŋ] | 'that thing there' | |
/dedan/ | [ⁿdeⁿdan] | 'dive, swim underwater' | |
/mamʷe/ | [mamʷe] | 'father' |
/oʔo/ | [oʔo] | 'yes' | |
/nomomox/ | [nomomox] | 'woman' | |
/noto/ | [noto] | 'chicken' |
/alimin/ | [alɪmɪn] | 'fifth' | |
/mavis/ | [mafɪs ~ mavɪs] | 'white' | |
/naʔanian/ | [naʔani(j)an] | 'food' |
The forms listed below provide contrast between the five vowel phonemes in the Neveʻei language (Musgrave, 2007, p. 13):
/i/ | /bir/ | 'fart audibly' | |
/u/ | /bur-/ | '1DU.IRR (verbal prefix)' | |
/e/ | /ber/ | 'long, tall, deep' | |
/o/ | /bor/ | 'deaf' | |
/a/ | /bar/ | 'blind' |
The table below shows the various syllable structures attested in Neveʻei (Musgrave, 2007, p. 13-14):
V (vowel), C (consonant) | Neveʻei | English | |
---|---|---|---|
V | /u-//i-/ | '2SG.REAL(verbal prefix)''3SG.REAL(verbal prefix)' | |
VC | /im//aɾ/ | 'only, just''they, them (pronoun)' | |
VCC | /itl/ | 'three' | |
CV | /vi/ | 'do, make' | |
CC | /tn/ | 'roast' | |
CCV | /nsu//tno/ | 'remove bark of''my (pronoun)' | |
CCVC | /nsev//tlel/ | 'cough''secret' | |
CVC | /dan//veh/ | 'go down, sink''carry' | |
CVCC | /matl//bans/ | 'thick''wander' | |
CCVCC | /nsutl/ | 'eight' |
Syllables that consist of two vowels are also present, but are only used for specific reasons (Musgrave, 2007, p. 13). While most two non-like vowels can be used to form a morpheme, there are certain exceptions that must be noted (Musgrave, 2007, p. 13).
Impermissible vowel sequences[6] | ||
---|---|---|
eo, oe | mid vowels cannot be followed by another mid vowel | |
ea | mid front vowel /e/ cannot be followed by /a/ | |
iu, eu | front vowels /i/ and /e/ cannot be followed by /u/ | |
ae, ao | low vowel /a/ cannot be followed by a mid vowel | |
ui | back vowel /u/ cannot be followed by /i/ |
Phonotactic patterns display various possibilities within morphemes (Musgrave, 2007, p. 14).
In root-initial forms, any of the vowels are possible but initial vowels are not common in lexical items. Initial vowels, as displayed below, are found mostly in grammatical items, for example (Musgrave, 2007, p. 14):
/i/ | 'he/she/it' | |
/en/ | 'instrumental' | |
/aɾ/ | 'they, them' | |
/oʔo/ | 'yes' | |
/utnaŋ/ | 'there' |
In Neveʻei, stress is predictable in which the primary stress lies on the penultimate syllable where the last two syllables contain single vowels, for example (Musgrave, 2007, p. 21):
/ˈvenox/ | 'steal' | |
/meˈtabux/ | 'morning' | |
/ˈmoxot/ | 'torn, ripped' | |
/ˈnowi/ | 'water' |
Neveʻei does not have a traditional orthography. Like elsewhere in Vanuatu, most speakers of Neveʻei write using either Bislama, English or sometimes French. However, Neveʻei is sometimes used in writing for certain special occasions, such as the composition of hymns, recording details of family history, and land ownership.[7] Due to the lack of stable spelling conventions for Neveʻei, individuals differ in the choice of orthographical methods when transcribing unusual phonemes. In the past efforts had been made to create a standard orthography, but the current situation is unknown.[7]
In Neveʻei, independent pronouns can function as verbal subjects, as verbal and prepositional objects, and as pronominal possessors following directly possessed nouns. Independent pronouns inflect according to person and number (singular, dual, or non-singular), but not according to case or grammatical gender. First person pronouns also distinguish between inclusive and exclusive in the dual and non-singular forms.[8]
Singular | Dual | Non-singular | ||
---|---|---|---|---|
1st inclusive | no | getdru | get | |
1st exclusive | gememru | gemem | ||
2nd | gu | gemru | gem | |
3rd | i | ardru | ar |
Although Neveʻei has a distinction between dual and plural, the forms listed as non-singular can be used to refer to both dual and plural subjects. In these cases, even when the non-singular independent pronoun is used, the dual verbal prefix will be used.[9]
Example:[10]
The pronominal forms listed as dual in the table are instead used in cases where there is a pragmatic contrast with singular or plural reference, or where two participants are acting together rather than separately.[11]
Example:
Neveʻei contains a set of possessive postmodifiers which are used as possessive adjuncts with indirectly possessed nouns. The possessive pronouns (used when the possessed noun is not overtly expressed) are derived from these possessive postmodifiers by adding the prefix ti-, although this may be omitted in some forms. Like the independent pronouns, these pronominal forms indicate person and number, and the non-singular forms may be used for dual referents. These pronominal forms also have an inclusive/exclusive distinction. Additionally, possessive postmodifiers can take singular, dual and non-singular forms, similar to independent pronouns, in which case the non-singular form is often employed for both dual and plural referents (Musgrave, 2007).
Singular | Dual | Non-singular | ||
---|---|---|---|---|
1st inclusive | tno | tegetdru | teget | |
1st excusive | tegememru | tegemem | ||
2nd | tugu | tegemru | tegem | |
3rd | ti | terdru | ter |
Singular | Dual | Non-singular | ||
---|---|---|---|---|
1st inclusive | titno | (ti)tegetdru | (ti)teget | |
1st excusive | (ti)tegememru | (ti)tegemem | ||
2nd | (ti)tugu | tegemru | (ti)tegem | |
3rd | titi | titerdru | titer |
Example:[13]
Neveʻei also contains a set of indefinite pronouns which function as both verbals subjects and objects, with separate forms in fast and slow speech.[14]
Slow speech | Fast speech | ||
---|---|---|---|
numuruan | numurwan | "somebody, someone" | |
nusutuan | nusutwan | "something" | |
nisituan | nisitwan | "something" | |
nei mang (mi) | "who, whoever" | ||
Neveʻei mang (mi) | "what, whatever" |
Example:[15]
In Neveʻei, similar to most Oceanic Languages, nouns do not change.[16] The inflection of nouns by numbers does not occur and instead postmodifiers are used. Only two types of possession exist in Neveʻei. These are direct and indirect. For a directly possessed noun, a suffix is attached directly to the noun itself and for an indirectly possessed noun, a possessive postmodifier appears before the noun.
Most often, directly possessed nouns have stems ending in vowels, however there are also instances in which stems ending in non-nasal alveolar consonants t, s, l and r occur (Musgrave, 2007). The following table provides examples of both instances (Musgrave, 2007 p. 33):
Vowel Endings | Consonant Endings | |
---|---|---|
khava - 'brother, friend' | nagalel - 'rib, side (of body) | |
ninsibi - 'finger, toe claw (of bird/chicken)' | netal - 'foot, leg, behind leg (of animal)' | |
nobologo - 'bone' | nibis - 'tail' |
In Neveʻei complex nouns can be formed by compounding or affixation.[18] Compounding involves combining a noun root with either a noun or verb root. And affixation involves deriving nouns from verbs, a process in which a simulfix is added to the verb stem.
Neveʻei uses a decimal (base 10) system for numbers, however many young people are only familiar with one to ten, so higher value numerals are often expressed using methods derived from Bislama.[19]
10 is an interesting number in Neveʻei because it can be either or .[20] Also note that is used for the actual number 10 when counting and vungavil is used to modify a phrase or noun. An example would be noang vungavil which translates to "10 canoes" in English.In Neveʻei, subject verbal prefixes are obligatory and indicate the person and number of the subject. There are two complete sets of prefixes: one for realis mood and the other for irrealis. Unlike the pronoun systems, there is no distinction between inclusive and exclusive first person, and dual prefixes are always used with dual referents. There is no set of affixes encoding the person and number of objects. Vowels in some of these prefixes may change according to the first vowel in the verb stem; this is a process of progressive assimilation at a distance.[21]
Realis | Irrealis | ||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Singular | Dual | Plural | Singular | Dual | Plural | ||
1st | nV- | er- | it- | nVbwV- | bwVr- | bwit- | |
2nd | u- | ar- | at- | kV- | abwVr- | abwit- | |
3rd | i-/∅- | bwV- |
Example:[23]
The third person singular realis prefix is sometimes realised as ∅- in various environments. This is more common with certain verbs, especially verbs where the stem begins with s.
Example:[24]
Verbal prefixes ending in t also often lose the t before verb roots beginning with s.
Example:
Reduplication is most commonly used in Neveʻei to indicate things such as intensity, prohibition, reciprocity and habitual aspects and is also sometimes used in order to differentiate between transitive and intransitive forms of a verb.[25] The most common method of reduplication in Neveʻei is the repeating of the first syllable in the verb stem.
This example shows reduplication to express a habitual aspect.
This example shows reduplication to express prohibition.
This example shows reduplication to express reciprocity.
This example shows reduplication to express intensity.
In Neveʻei transitive and intransitive verbs are represented by a technique in which intransitive verbs become transitive through the addition of a suffix -V(vowel)n to the end of an intransitive verb stem.[26]
The suffix to make verbs transitive has three allomorphs. They are:
Intransitive | Transitive | |
---|---|---|
nonong "finish" | nonong-on ~ nonong-en "finish" | |
ngang "laugh" | ngang-an ~ ngang-en "laugh at" | |
dedan "dive" | dedan-en "dive for" |
One other way is for both transitive and intransitive verbs to have very different forms of expression.
Intransitive | Transitive | ||
---|---|---|---|
"eat" | ʻaʻan | khan | |
"steal" | vevenaʻ | venokh | |
"burn" | lililmin | vang do |
Transitive[28] | Intransitive | ||
---|---|---|---|
"turn around" | vilih | vil-vilih | |
"buy" | vul | vul-vul | |
"drink" | min | min-min | |
"copulate (with)" | ʻav | ʻav-ʻav |
In Neveʻei negation is marked by the simulfixes 'not' and 'not yet'. The prefixed element of the negative marker directly follows the subject prefix and precedes the verb stem while the suffixed element is always the final morpheme of the verb.[29] The prefixed element sV- is used in both negative simulfixes with the realisation of V- depending on a process of vowel harmony. The suffixed element -si remains the same following both transitive and intransitive verbs, however the element from the other simulfix is following intransitive verbs and (here -an can be described as a realisation of the transitive suffix -Vn) after transitive verbs.[30]
The following examples[31] show how the negative simulfixes are used with the intransitive verb nonong 'finish' and the transitive equivalent nonong-on 'finish (something)'.
When a sentence containing a verb with the completive suffix -i is negated the simulfix usually replaces the completive suffix as in the following example where mah-i becomes mah-vang-an in the negative version.[32]
Neveʻei allows for inflected verb stems to consist of a sequence of two or more stems. These verbs in sequence are negated as a single unit with the negative prefix preceding the first verb stem and the suffix following the final verb stem in the sequence.[33]
For example:[34]
The following verbs in Neveʻei are attested as both as independent verbs and as serial verbs expressing aspect.[35]
'progressive' | ||
mera` | 'inceptive' | |
tokh | 'habitual' | |
vwer | 'actual' |
Neveʻei also contains the following verbs that are attested as both independent verbs and serial verbs expressing modality.[38]
'abilitative' | ||
yangwal | 'desiderative' | |
sisi | 'anti-desiderative' | |
vwer | 'intentional' |
Even though 'desiderative' can be negated as seen in the previous example there is also the separate verb sisi that expresses anti-desiderative modality. As is the case when it appears as an aspectual auxiliary also cannot be negated when used to express intentional modality instead sisi is used to express negative intention.[40]
To negate directional serial constructions only the first verb carries the simulfix as in the following example where vavu 'walk' carries the negation and the directional verb vu 'thither' appears in the affirmative.[41]
There are five determiners in Neveʻei: the proximate demonstrative nene, the intermediate demonstrative nenana, and the distant demonstrative nenokhoi, the anaphoric demonstrative nge, and the indefinite postmodifier tuan.
Proximate | Intermediate | Distant | Anaphoric | |
---|---|---|---|---|
nene | nenang | nenokhoi | nge | |
'this' | 'that' | 'that' | 'the' | |
near | middle distance | far away | mentioned previously |
Much like the vast majority of languages around Vanuatu and neighbouring Austronesian speaking regions, Neveʻei follows a Noun-Demonstrative pattern,[43] [44] where a demonstrative follows the noun head and any adjectival or possessive postmodifiers.
Both a three-way distinction and the Noun-Demonstrative pattern are thought to be features retained by Neveʻei from Proto-Oceanic.[45]
In Neveʻei, nouns with indefinite reference are given the indefinite postmodifier tuan, while nouns with definite reference are either given the appropriate demonstrative or left unmarked. Musgrave suggests that since many Neveʻei nouns begin with nV, the Proto-Oceanic article *na attached to the noun and became a part of the noun root, meaning speakers no longer needed to mark the definite article.
The three non-anaphoric forms of the demonstrative have the same initial element ne-, similar to how nouns often appear in the language. The demonstratives also have final forms identical to how Neveʻei indicates other locational forms which also have three-way distinctions, for example, utne, 'in this place, here', utnang, 'in that place, there', and utnokhoi, 'in that place, over there'.
The demonstrative nene indicates that the noun is close to the speaker.
The demonstrative nenang is most commonly used to mark temporal orientation rather than spatial.
The demonstrative nge is an anaphoric demonstrative meaning it is used to refer to a noun that has already been introduced. It is identical in form to the pronominal trace, which marks the original site of a noun phrase in a relative clause.
The anaphoric demonstrative cannot only be used after a noun has been marked by tuan to be used in later references. In the example above, natuturmwitiyilian 'story' is first modified by a relative clause marked by nen, then later referred to with the anaphoric demonstrative.
Grammatically, the anaphoric demonstrative nge behaves differently from the three other demonstratives in Neveʻei in that it can only ever function as a nominal postmodifier, while the other determiners can occur as both nominal postmodifiers and nominal phrase heads. This example shows the proximate demonstrative nene used as the head of a nominal phrase:
Spatial deictics are common in Neveʻei, especially in adverbials which share endings with a corresponding demonstrative:[46]
Each of the final elements of both forms of these adverbs are identical to the demonstratives nene 'this', nenang 'that', and nenokhoi, 'that, over there'. In the second adverbial in each category takes the form utnen 'place where' followed by the proximate, intermediate, or distant form ie, iang, or yokhoi.In the following examples, a place adverbial appears in the clause-final position as a non-core argument:
Both the adverbial and the locative adverb are acceptable in Neveʻei, as shown in the first example where the locative adverb utne is used to mean 'here' and the second example, which uses the adverbial utnen ie.
Place adverbials can also occur in a clause-initial position:
The adverbials listed above can be combined with place adverbs to form new meanings. For example; the adverbs ra'ai 'above' and retan 'below', which provide information about vertical location, such as speaking from a high position like the top of a hill or a low position like at the base of a tree.
Proximate | Intermediate | Distant | |
---|---|---|---|
ra'ai utne / ra'ai utnen ie 'up here' | ra'ai utnang / ra'ai utnen iang 'up there' | ra'ai utnokhoi / ra'ai utnen yokhoi 'way up there' | |
retan utne / retan utnen ie 'down here' | retan utnang / retan utnen iang 'down there | retan utnokhain / retan utnen yokhoi 'way down there' |
The proximate and intermediate final elements of the proximate and intermediate demonstratives nene 'this' and nenang 'that' are also shared by the locational and temporal forms ie 'this here', iang 'that there', itie 'now, at this time', and itiang 'then, at that time'.
Extending on directly possessed noun stems in section 4.2. Nouns, when these directly possessed nouns appear to have a nominal possessor that is when the directly possessed nouns do not refer to a particular possessor, a suffix which is identical to the person singular pronominal suffix –n, known as a construct suffix (Musgrave, 2007). The construct suffixes are depicted in the table below:
After consonant | After vowels | ||
---|---|---|---|
1st person | -ung/-ing | -ng | |
2nd person | -um/im | -m | |
3rd person | -n | -n |
The construct suffix –ing/im is attached to –iC (where C is consonant) root stems, whereas –ung/um suffixes follow noun stem roots ending in all other consonants. Additionally, noun roots that end in a vowel have the suffix –ng/-m attached.
Some examples from Musgrave's (2007, p. 34) grammar include:
Unlike many other languages closely related to Neveʻei, the patterns for directly suffixed nouns are not as complicated. As many of the closely related languages have distinct pronominal suffixes that correspond to both singular and non-singular pronouns independent pronouns, Neveʻei is moving away from a rigid difference between how pronominal and nominal possession is demonstrated (Musgrave, 2007).
According to Musgrave's (2007) grammar findings, when found without a reference to a specific possessor, directly possessed nouns are displayed in their construct form, and the third person non-singular independent pronoun –ar and the plural postmodifier –ar are identical, their semantics must be worked out in context. For instance, depending on the context nat-n ar could be translated to 'the children', with the plural postmodifier, or 'their children', possession (Musgrave, 2007, p. 35).
E.g.:
However, determining the differences between the uses of the singular pronominal possessor constructions, it has been found that pronominal suffix formations are used more often for third person singular possessors and for first person singular possessors as a term of address, in narratives (Musgrave, 2007).
Extending on aforementioned possessive pronoun information, when the nominal phrase head is taken by a possessive pronoun, the postmodifiers used are determiners and plural postmodifiers. This is explained in the following formula:
POSSESSIVE PRONOUN + (DETERMINER) + (PLURAL POSTMODIFIER)
For example, the possessive pronouns precedes the determiner nge:
(Musgrave, 2007, p. 61)
When indefinite pronouns inhabit the nominal phrase head, possessive postmodifiers, determiners and plural postmodifiers are the only postmodifiers employed. As such, they follow the following formula:
INDEFINITE PRONOUN + (POSSESSIVE POSTMODIFIER) + (DETERMINER) + (PLURAL POSTMODIFIER)
Musgrave (2007, p. 62) demonstrates an example of this construction as follows: the indefinite pronoun nusutwan precedes the possessive postmodifier ter and the plural postmodifier ar.
Indirectly possessed nouns are unable to take a possessive suffix, and therefore adopt possessive modifier forms instead. An example of this is found in Musgrave's (2007) grammar.
Musgrave's (2007) grammar book identifies that Neveʻei, like most languages, has two main types of possession, direct and indirect possession. The constructions of possession show how the language has deviated from typical rules of grammar from Proto Oceanic language system and other languages of Vanuatu. A main difference identified in Musgrave's (2007) grammar is that Neveʻei does not juxtapose different indirect possessive subtypes and its direct possessive forms are reduced to singular pronominal possessors, as opposed to the Proto Oceanic system.
As previously discussed, if a directly possessed noun has a nominal possessor, in its construct form, a possessor follows the directly possessed nouns. These directly possessed nouns have a very close semantic relationship between the referent of the possessed and possessor nouns, referred to as inalienable possession (Musgrave, 2007). An inalienable relationship describes a situation when the possessed noun does not exist separately from the possessor. This notion is exhibited by the following formula:
POSSESSED NOUN + POSSESSOR NOUN + (POSSESSOR NOUN MODIFIERS) + (POSSESSED NOUN POSTMODIFIERS)
Consequently, no postmodifiers belonging to the directly possessed noun can come between it and the nominal possessor.
Most often the categories of alienable possessive constructions relate to kin/relationship terms, parts of a plant, body parts of humans and animals etc. Examples from Musgrave (2007, p. 71) are listed below.
Neveʻei | English | ||
---|---|---|---|
Kinship | na'aibi-nnat-n tabi-n | 'grandchild''child' 'grandfather | |
Animal/human body parts | na'adle-nna-avera-n neleme-n | 'egg (of bird or turtle, roe (of fish)''wing' 'tongue' | |
Plant parts | na'ansemwe-nnokhora-n netevi-n | 'stem (of leaf, fruit)'root' 'shoot (of a plant)' | |
Bodily products | nemakha-nnemwetebwe-n | 'urine''excrement' |
(Musgrave, 2007, p. 72)
When an indirectly possessed noun has a pronominal possessor, it precedes a possessive postmodifier. However, there are two outcomes for a possessive construction when an indirectly possessed noun takes a nominal possessor.
In the first construction the possessive postmodifier agrees with the number of the possessor noun, establishing a semantic relationship that is distant or alienable, as the possessed noun exists independently of the possessor (adnominal possession). For example, (Musgrave, 2007, p. 73):
POSSESSED NOUN + POSSESSIVE POSTMODIFIER + POSSESSOR NOUN + (POSTMODIFIERS)
POSSESSOR NOUN + (POSTMODIFIERS) + POSSESSED NOUN + POSSESSIVE POSTMODIFIER
Another indirect possessive construction includes a possessive marker nen, which acts similarly to a relativiser. The formula demonstrates that nen is found in between the possessed nominal and the possessor nominal.
POSSESSED NOUN + NEN + POSSESSOR NOUN + (POSSESSOR POSTMODIFIERS) + (POSSESSED POSTMODIFIERS)
These constructions often pertain to a certain semantic category: purposive relationships, habitual relations and possession of body parts. In the following example from Musgrave (2007, p. 75), the possessed noun is a person and the possessor noun in an occupation:
While some body parts are directly possessed nouns, some body parts (such as internal organs, bodily exudations etc.) are indirectly possessed nouns and most often pertain to an alienable semantic relationship.
(Musgrave, 2007, p. 75)
NONSG:non-singular1NONSG:first person, non-singular2NONSG:second person, non-singular3NONSG:third person, non-singularABIL:abilitativeDESID:desiderative