Kusunda | |
Also Known As: | Kusanda |
Nativename: | mihaq |
Pronunciation: | pronounced as /kgg/ |
States: | Nepal |
Region: | Gandaki Province, Lumbini Province |
Ethnicity: | 270 Kusunda (2011 census) |
Speakers: | 1 |
Date: | 2022 |
Ref: | e26 |
Script: | Devanagari |
Familycolor: | Isolate |
Family: | Language isolate |
Iso3: | kgg |
Glotto: | kusu1250 |
Glottorefname: | Kusunda |
Map: | Kusunda_language.png |
Mapcaption: | Ethnologue locations: (west) Dang and Pyuthan districts (dark grey) within Lumbini Province; (center) Tanahun District within Gandaki Province EndangeredLanguages.com location: red WALS location: purple (Gorkha District) |
Notice: | IPA |
Kusunda or Kusanda (endonym pronounced as /kgg/) is a language isolate spoken by a few among the Kusunda people in western and central Nepal. As of 2023, it only has a single fluent speaker, Kamala Khatri Sen,[1] although there are efforts underway to keep the language alive.[2]
For decades the Kusunda language was thought to be on the verge of extinction, with little hope of ever knowing it well. The little material that could be gleaned from the memories of former speakers suggested that the language was an isolate, but, without much evidence, it was often classified along with its neighbors as Tibeto-Burman. However in 2004 three Kusundas, Gyani Maya Sen, Prem Bahadur Shahi and Kamala Singh,[3] were brought to Kathmandu for help with citizenship papers. There, members of Tribhuvan University discovered that one of them, a native of Sakhi VDC in southern Rolpa District, was a fluent speaker of the language. Several of her relatives were also discovered to be fluent. In 2005 there were known to be seven or eight fluent speakers of the language, the youngest in her thirties. However the language is moribund, with no children learning it, since all Kusunda speakers have married outside their ethnicity.
It was presumed that the language became extinct with the death of Rajamama Kusunda on 19 April 2018.[4] However Gyani Maiya Sen and her sister Kamala Kusunda survived him and further data were collected.[5] The sisters, together with author and researcher Uday Raj Aaley, have been teaching the language to interested children and adults.[6]
Aaley, the facilitator and Kusunda-language teacher, has written the book Kusunda Tribe and Dictionary.[7] The book has a compilation of more than 1000 words from the Kusunda language.
David E. Watters published a mid-sized grammatical description of the language, plus vocabulary (Watters 2005), although further works have been published since.[8] He argued that Kusunda is indeed a language isolate, not just genealogically but also lexically, grammatically and phonologically distinct from its neighbors. This would imply that Kusunda is a remnant of the languages spoken in northern India before the influx of Tibeto-Burman- and Indo-Iranian-speaking peoples; however it is not classified as a Munda nor a Dravidian language. It thus joins Burushaski, Nihali and (potentially) the substrate of the Vedda language in the list of South Asian languages that do not fall into the main categories of Indo-European, Dravidian, Sino-Tibetan, and Austroasiatic.
Before the recent discovery of active Kusunda speakers there had been several attempts to link the language to an established language family. B.K. Rana (2002) maintained that Kusunda was a Tibeto-Burman language as traditionally classified. Merritt Ruhlen argued for a relationship with Juwoi and other Andamanese languages; and for a larger Indo-Pacific language family, with them and other languages, including Nihali.[9]
Others have linked Kusunda to Munda (see Watters 2005); Yeniseian (Gurov 1989); Burushaski and Caucasian (Reinhard and Toba 1970; this would be a variant of Gurov's proposal if Sino-Caucasian were accepted); and the Nihali isolate in central India (Fleming 1996, Whitehouse 1997). More recently a relationship between Kusunda, Yeniseian and Burushaski has been proposed.[10]
Phonetically, Kusunda has six vowels in two harmonic groups, which are arguably three vowels phonemically: a word will normally have vowels from the upper (pink, italic) or lower (green) set, but not both simultaneously. There are very few words that consistently have either always upper or always lower vowels; most words may be pronounced either way, though those with uvular consonants require the lower set (as in many languages). There are a few words with no uvular consonants that still bar such dual pronunciations, though these generally only feature the distinction in careful enunciation.
Close | pronounced as /ink/ | pronounced as /ink/ | ||
---|---|---|---|---|
Mid | pronounced as /ink/ | pronounced as /ink/ | pronounced as /ink/ | |
Open | pronounced as /ink/ |
Kusunda consonants seem to only contrast the active articulator, not where that articulator makes contact. For example, apical consonants may be dental, alveolar, retroflex, or palatal: pronounced as //t// is dental pronounced as /[t̪]/ before pronounced as //i//, alveolar pronounced as /[t͇]/ before pronounced as //e, ə, u//, retroflex pronounced as /[ʈ]/ before pronounced as //o, a//, and palatal pronounced as /[c]/ when there is a following uvular, as in pronounced as /[coq]/ ~ pronounced as /[t͇ok]/ ('we').
In addition, many consonants vary between stops and fricatives; for instance, pronounced as //p// seems to surface as pronounced as /[b]/ between vowels, while pronounced as //b// surfaces as pronounced as /[β]/ in the same environment. Aspiration appears to be recent to the language. Kusunda also lacks the retroflex consonant phonemes that are common to the region, and is unique in the region in having uvular consonants.
Labial | Coronal | Palatal | Velar | Uvular | Glottal | ||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Nasal | pronounced as /link/ | pronounced as /link/ | pronounced as /link/ | pronounced as /ink/ | |||||
Stop | voiceless | pronounced as /link/~pronounced as /ink/ | pronounced as /link/~pronounced as /ink/ | pronounced as /link/ | pronounced as /link/~pronounced as /ink/ | pronounced as /ink/~pronounced as /ink/ | pronounced as /link/ | ||
voiced | pronounced as /ink/~pronounced as /ink/ | pronounced as /ink/ | pronounced as /link/ | pronounced as /link/~pronounced as /ink/ | |||||
aspirated | (pronounced as /link/) | (pronounced as /link/) | (pronounced as /link/) | (pronounced as /link/~pronounced as /ink/) | (pronounced as /link/) | ||||
breathy | (pronounced as /link/) | (pronounced as /link/) | (pronounced as /link/) | (pronounced as /link/) | |||||
Fricative | pronounced as /link/ | pronounced as /ink/~pronounced as /ink/ | pronounced as /link/ | ||||||
Approximant | pronounced as /ink/ | pronounced as /link/ | pronounced as /link/ | ||||||
Flap | pronounced as /ink/ |
Kusunda has several cases, marked on nouns and pronouns, three of which are the nominative (Kusunda, unlike its neighbors, has no ergativity), genitive, and accusative.[11]
Nominative | Genitive | Accusative | ||
---|---|---|---|---|
1st person, singular | tsi | tsi, tsi-yi | tən-da | |
1st person, plural | tok | tig-i | (toʔ-da) | |
2nd person, singular | nu | nu, ni-yi | nən-da | |
2nd person, plural | nok | ?nig-i | (noʔ-da) | |
3rd person | gina | (gina-yi) | gin-da |
Other case suffixes include -ma "together with", -lage "for", -əna "from", -ga, -gə "at, in".
There are also demonstrative pronouns na and ta. Although it is not clear what the difference between them is, it may be animacy.
Subjects may be marked on the verb, though when they are they may either be prefixed or suffixed. An example with am "eat", which is more regular than many verbs, in the present tense (-ən) is,
1st person | t-əm-ən | t-əm-da-n | |
---|---|---|---|
2nd person | n-əm-ən | n-əm-da-n | |
3rd person | g-əm-ən | g-əm-da-n |
Other verbs may have a prefix ts- in the first person, or zero in the third.
Proto-Kusunda | |
Familycolor: | Isolate |
Target: | Kusunda language |
Proto-Kusunda pre-root nominal prefixes can be categorized into a two=slot system, with the possessor prefix attached before the classificatory prefix, which in turn comes before the root noun (for example, *g-u-hu 'bone' and *g-i-dzi 'name').
possessor prefix (-2) | classificatory prefix (-1) | ||
---|---|---|---|
1st person |
|
| |
2nd person |
|
| |
3rd person |
|
|
The proposed class markers *i-, *a-, *u-, and *ja- are proposed to be triggered by the possessive-marking prefixes *t-, *n-, and *g-. The system is reminiscent of nominal morphology in the Great Andamanese languages.
Below are some Proto-Kusunda lexical reconstructions from Spendley (2024),[12] based on data of different Kusunda dialects from from Hodgson (1857) and Reinhard & Toba (1970).[13] [14]
gloss | Proto-Kusunda | |
---|---|---|
arm |
| |
below |
| |
blood |
| |
bone |
| |
child |
| |
ear |
| |
eye |
| |
father |
| |
foot, leg |
| |
friend |
| |
hole |
| |
knee |
| |
mother-in-law |
| |
mouth |
| |
name |
| |
nose |
| |
skin |
| |
stomach |
| |
tongue |
| |
tooth |
|