Crow | |
Nativename: | Apsáalooke aliláau |
States: | USA |
Region: | Montana |
Ethnicity: | 8,500 Crow (2007)[1] |
Speakers: | 4,160 (2015) |
Familycolor: | American |
Fam1: | Siouan |
Fam2: | Western Siouan |
Fam3: | Missouri River Siouan |
Iso3: | cro |
Glotto: | crow1244 |
Lingua: | 64-AAA-b |
Glottorefname: | Crow |
Notice: | IPA |
Map: | Lang Status 60-DE.svg |
Root: | Apsáalooke "children of the large beaked bird" |
People: | Apsáalooke |
Language: | Apsáalooke aliláau Apsáalooke iiéhkuua |
Country: | Apsáalooke Isawúua |
Crow (native name: Apsáalooke in Siouan languages pronounced as /ə̀ˈpsâːɾòːɡè/ or in Siouan languages pronounced as /ə̀ˈpsâːlòːɡè/) is a Missouri Valley Siouan language spoken primarily by the Crow Nation in present-day southeastern Montana. The word Apsáalooke translates to "Children of the Large Beaked Bird" (from apá 'beak/nose', isáa 'big', dooká 'child'), which was later incorrectly translated into English as 'Crow'.[2] It is one of the larger populations of American Indian languages with 4,160 speakers according to the 2015 US Census.[3]
Crow is closely related to Hidatsa spoken by the Hidatsa tribe of the Dakotas; the two languages are the only members of the Missouri Valley Siouan family.[4] [5] Despite their similarities, Crow and Hidatsa are not mutually intelligible.
According to Ethnologue with figures from 1998, 77% of Crow people over 66 years old speak the language; "some" parents and older adults, "few" high school students and "no pre-schoolers" speak Crow. 80% of the Crow Nation prefers to speak in English.[1] The language was defined as "definitely endangered" by UNESCO as of 2012.[6]
However, R. Graczyk claims in his A Grammar of Crow (2007) that "[u]nlike many other native languages of North America in general, and the northern plain in particular, the Crow language still exhibits considerable vitality: there are fluent speakers of all ages, and at least some children are still acquiring Crow as their first language." Many of the younger population who do not speak Crow are able to understand it. Almost all of those who do speak Crow are also bilingual in English.[7] Graczyk cites the reservation community as the reason for both the high level of bilingual Crow-English speakers and the continued use and prevalence of the Crow language. Daily contact with non–American Indians on the reservation for over one hundred years has led to high usage of English. Traditional culture within the community, however, has preserved the language via religious ceremonies and the traditional clan system.
Currently, most speakers of Crow are 30 and older but a few younger speakers are learning it. There are increased efforts for children to learn Crow as their first language and many do on the Crow Reservation of Montana, particularly through a Crow language immersion school that was sponsored in 2012.[8] Development for the language includes a Crow language dictionary and portions of the Bible published from 1980-2007. The current literacy rate is around 1-5% for first language speakers and 75-100% for second language learners.[9] Teens are immersed in Crow at the Apsaalooke language camp sponsored by the Crow Nation.[10] [11]
Crow is closely related to Hidatsa spoken by the Hidatsa tribe of the Dakotas; the two languages are the only members of the Missouri Valley Siouan family.[4] [5] The ancestor of Crow-Hidatsa may have constituted the initial split from Proto-Siouan. Crow and Hidatsa are not mutually intelligible, however the two languages share many phonological features, cognates and have similar morphologies and syntax. The split between Crow and Hidatsa may have occurred between 300 and 800 years ago.[12] [13] [14]
There are five distinct vowels in Crow, which occur either long or short with the exception of the mid vowels.
Short | Long | ||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|
Front | Back | Front | Back | ||
High (close) | pronounced as /i/ | pronounced as /u/ | pronounced as /iː/ | pronounced as /uː/ | |
Mid | pronounced as /eː/ | pronounced as /oː/ | |||
Low (open) | pronounced as /a/ | pronounced as /aː/ | |||
Diphthong | pronounced as /ia/ | pronounced as /ua/ |
There is also a marginal diphthong ea pronounced as /[ea]/ that only occurs in two native Crow stems: déaxa 'clear' and béaxa 'intermittent'.
Crow has a very sparse consonant inventory, much like many other languages of the Great Plains.
Labial | Alveolar | Palatal | Velar | Glottal | ||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Plosive | pronounced as /link/ | pronounced as /link/ | pronounced as /link/ | pronounced as /link/ | (pronounced as /link/) | |
Fricative | pronounced as /link/ | pronounced as /link/ | pronounced as /link/ | pronounced as /link/ | ||
Sonorant | pronounced as /link/~pronounced as /link/~pronounced as /link/ | pronounced as /link/~pronounced as /link/~pronounced as /link/ |
Stops are aspirated word-initially, word-finally, when geminated (e.g. [ppʰ]) and when following another stop (e.g. [ptʰ]). Stops in a consonant cluster with h as the initial radical (hp, ht, hk) are unaspirated and lax. Gemination in stops only occurs intervocalically. Intervocalic single, nongeminate stops are lax, unaspirated, and generally voiced. The difference between voiced stops b and d (allophones of m and n) and voiceless stops is hardly discernible when following a fricative, since both are unaspirated and lax. The phoneme k has a palatalized allophone [kʲ] that occurs after i, e, ch and sh, often word-finally.
Fricatives are tense; they are only lax when intervocalic. Palatal sh is often voiced intervocalically; s is sometimes voiced intervocalically; x is never voiced. The alveolar fricative /s/ has an optional allophone /h/ in phrase-initial position:
Sonorants voiced /m/ and /n/ have three allophones: w and l intervocalically, b and d word initially and following an obstruent, and m and n in all other conditions. In conservative speech, l is realized as a tapped r, however in general cases it is realized as l, perhaps due in part to the influence of English. Word initially, b is optional for /m/, though b is more commonly realized. The glottal sonorant /h/ assimilates to the nasality of the following segment, but retains its voicelessness. When following i or e or preceding ch, /h/ may be realized as an alveopalatal fricative.
Vowel sequences across morpheme boundaries can be quite varied, but short vowels cannot appear alone in the morpheme: V:V (long+short), V:V: (long+long) and diphthong+V (short). Word finally, only a (in a diphthong), o, and u (allomorphs of the plural suffix) can occur after a long vowel.
A wide variety of consonant clusters can occur in Crow. All consonants except for /h/ can be geminated. Voiced labials and dentals (phonemic m and n, allophones b, m, w and d, n, l) are resistant to clustering. Because they only occur intervocalically, l and w do not occur in clusters. The plosive allophones b and d only occur in clusters as the second consonant and only at morpheme boundaries. The nasal allophones m and n can only occur with each other with the exception of nm, or occur with h at a morpheme boundary. Clusters in general occur at morphemic boundaries.
Some morphemic constraints:
Stress in Crow is phonemic. The position of the stress in the stem is determined lexically. Virtually all noun and verb stems have an inherent stress. In word initial syllables, accented short vowels in a word initial syllable are generally followed by a consonant cluster, while accented long vowels are generally followed by a single consonant. Stress can fall on short vowels as well as long vowels and may fall on either mora of a long vowel. With diphthongs, either the long vowel or the offglide may bear the stress.
Stress helps predict the tones of all the vowels in a word: stressed vowels are high in pitch; all vowels following the stressed vowel are low in pitch; all short vowels preceding the stressed vowel are low in pitch; all long vowels preceding the stressed vowel are high in pitch; short vowels occurring between a long vowel and the accented vowel assimilate to a high pitch.
In words composed of more than one morpheme, there are several rules (with a few exceptions) to determine the placement of the stress:
Exceptions:
Phonological processes in Crow include:
Crow is a polysynthetic language.
Basic stems consist of one to four syllables (with four being rare) and always end in a vowel. Monosyllabic stems have long vowels or diphthongs, e.g., bií, 'stone, rock'; bía, 'woman'. The vast majority of nouns in Crow are derived stems. Derivational processes in nominal morphology include affixation and compounding.
An exhaustive list of nominal suffixes:
Prefixes will render a relative clause into a derived noun.
There are two basic types of compounding in Crow: noun-noun compounds and noun-verb compounds.
Noun-noun compounds often involve a whole-part relationship: the first noun refers to the whole and the second to the part. Members of the compound may also be themselves compounds or derived nouns.
Noun-verb compounds consist of a noun plus a stative verb. There are a number of select exceptions.
Nouns are classified as either inalienably or alienably possessed, according to which possessive markers they occur with.
Inalienably possessed nouns are those that are inherently possessed or nondetachable associations, specifically body parts and family members, opposed to alienably possessed nouns whose entity is not inherently possessed. This rule is not absolute as some body parts and kin nouns can be considered alienable and some nouns with close associations to its possessor (i.e., 'his house', 'her dog') can be considered inalienable.
The affixed possession paradigm for inalienable and alienable possessives can be derived. The alienable possessives only use the first consonant of the alienable prefixes and do not mark the possessor when the prefix begins with a vowel. The final suffix transforms into a diphthong from /-o/.
Alienable | Inalienable | ||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|
Singular | Plural | Singular | Plural | ||
1st Person | bas- | bas--o | b- | b--úua | |
2nd Person | dís- | dís--o | d- | d--uua | |
3rd Person | is- | is--o | 0- | 0--úua |
Personal names constitute a distinct morphological class of nouns in Crow. They are marked with the definite determiner suffix /sh/, which attaches to the stem rather than to the citation form.
Crow has three pronominal forms: bound; emphatic and contrastive; and interrogative-indefinite pronouns. With the first two types, there is a correlation between morphology and syntax. Argument pronouns are generally bound whereas emphatic and contrastive pronouns are generally independent. Bound pronominals function as direct and oblique arguments.
Bound Pronominal Stems:
A-Set | B-Set | ||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|
Singular | Plural | Singular | Plural | ||
1st Person | baa | baa+PL | bii | balee | |
2nd Person | dá(a) | dá(a)+PL | dii | dii+PL | |
3rd Person | 0 | 0+PL | 0 | 0+PL |
Verbal derivational morphology is composed of prefixes, suffixes, one infix (chi, 'again; possessive reflexive') and reduplication, which expresses an "iterative, distributive, or intensive sense to the meaning of the stem."[15]
The morphological verb classes in Crow mirror a semantic distinction: Crow is an active–stative language, meaning that the subject of an active verb is treated differently than the subject of a stative verb. Active verbs and stative verbs are marked with distinct sets of pronominal affixes: the "A-set" for active verbs and the "B-set" for stative verbs.
Active verbs may have one, two, or three arguments (making them respectively intransitive, transitive, or ditransitive). An intransitive verb takes a subject (SV), a transitive verb takes a subject and an object (SOV) and a ditransitive verb takes a subject and two objects (SO1O2V). In a relative clause built on an active verb, when the subject of the verb is the head of the relative clause and it is an animate noun phrase, it is marked by ak.
Stative verbs may have zero (impersonal), one, or two arguments. In a relative clause, the subject of a stative verb is marked with m or in elevated discourse, dak. There may also be an absence of marking on the head noun where the entire relative clause is marked with the indefinite nonspecific determiner m.
Crow has a fairly complex ordering of verb phrase constituents. The following table demonstrates simple constructions of active-state intransitive and transitive verbs based on the first person.
1 | 2 | 3 | 4 | 5 | 6 | 7 | 8 | 9 | 10 | 11 | ||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Indirect object | Indirect Object No. | Direct object | Subject: Transitive verb | Subject: Intransitive verb | Subject No. | Verb Stem | Subject: Transitive Causative | Causative | Subject No. | Mood | ||
B-Set Pronominals | A-Set Pronominals | |||||||||||
1 | baa | Active Intransitive | Singular-Plural | Mood | ||||||||
2 | bii | Singular-Plural | Active Intransitive | [b] | aa | Singular-Plural | Mood | |||||
3 | bii | baa | Singular-Plural | Active Transitive | Singular-Plural | Mood | ||||||
4 | bii | Singular-Plural | bii | Singular-Plural | Active Transitive | wa | hc | Singular-Plural | Mood | |||
5 | bii | Stative Intransitive | Singular-Plural | Mood | ||||||||
6 | bii | Singular-Plural | Stative Intransitive | Singular-Plural | Mood |
The verb chain constituents are, of course, much more complicated. Following is a concise list of the rank ordering of each type element:
Crow is a subject–object–verb (SOV) language; it is a verb-final and head marking. In noun phrases, the order is possessor–possessum, with the person marker of the possessor identified by a prefix to the possessum. Subordinate clauses precede matrix clauses, and are marked by a suffixed clause-final marker. Relative clauses are internally headed. Crow has postpositional phrases, with the postposition often occurring as a prefix to the following verb. There is no distinct category of adjectives; instead, stative verbs function as noun phrase modifiers.
Crow is an active–stative language, with verbs divided into two classes, active (both transitive and intransitive) and stative, largely on semantic grounds. This is also often called a "split intransitive" language.
An analysis of Crow noun phrase syntax under generative grammar has yielded the following rules:
There are two phrases that are subordinate to the NP (noun-phrase): (1) the DP (demonstrative phrase) and (2) the QP (quantifier phrase).
A noun phrase can be marked as definite or indefinite by a suffixed determiner (DET). The definite suffix is /-sh/ and the indefinite suffix is /-m/.
The determiner suffix is attached to the final word of the noun phrase, not just the agentive noun.
N' → [s...N' head...]
N' → NP N'
N' → PP N'
Q → DP QThere are two classes of quantifiers that are distinguished syntactically. The first class heads a quantifier phrases that takes a demonstrative (or in its stead, a noun phrase) as its complement: xaxúa.
The second class is a stative verb that may function as a nominal modifier. This class includes: ahú 'many, much', hawa 'some', kooshtá 'few', sáawi 'how many, so many, some', and the numerals. This class may also be followed by a determiner. They may also function as clausal predicates.
Q → DP QDemonstratives are deictic words; in Crow, they occur phrase-initially. They can also cooccur with determiners (ex. 'this the horse').
NP → NP NP/ko/ (demonstrative) and /kon/ (appositive) are used to modify each other.
Relative clauses in Crow are complex and subject to theoretical debates. There are two types of relative clauses in Crow: lexically headed and non-lexically headed. There are two basic relativizers /ak/ and /ala/, several composite forms based on ala plus baa 'indefinite pronoun' and instances with no relativizer. /ak/ indicates the subject of the relative clause is relativized and marks the subject as animate, and generally agentive. It can occur in both lexically and non-lexically headed clauses. /ala/ may indicate a locative, temporal or manner adverbial is the head of the relative clause. In non-lexically headed relative clauses, /ala/ can sometimes be interpreted as the head of the clause itself. It can also occur in both lexically and non-lexically headed clauses. The relativizers are bound, with many exception, but they are generally prefixed to the word that contains the verb of the relative clause.
Relative clauses are marked with final determiners. If the definite referent of the relative clause has already been accounted in the discourse or is otherwise obvious, the relative clause is marked with the definite /-sh/. Relative clauses can also be marked with the indefinite determiner marker /-m/; generally this is used to imply that the referent is being introduced into the discourse for the first time. However, the nominal head is almost always marked by the indefinite determiner /-m/.