Cognitive specialization explained

Cognitive specialization suggests that certain behaviors, often in the domain of social communication, are passed on to offspring and refined to be maximally beneficial by the process of natural selection. Specializations serve an adaptive purpose for an organism by allowing the organism to be better suited for its habitat. Over time, specializations often become essential to the species' continued survival. Cognitive specialization in humans has been thought to underlie the acquisition, development, and evolution of language, theory of mind, and specific social skills such as trust and reciprocity. These specializations are considered to be critical to the survival of the species, even though there are successful individuals who lack certain specializations, including those diagnosed with autism spectrum disorder or who lack language abilities.[1] Cognitive specialization is also believed to underlie adaptive behaviors such as self-awareness, navigation, and problem solving skills in several animal species such as chimpanzees and bottlenose dolphins.

Background

First studied as an adaptive mechanism specific to humans, cognitive specialization has since evolved to encompass many behaviors in the social realm. Organisms have evolved over millions of years to become well-adapted to their habitats; this requires becoming specialized in behaviors that improve an organism's likelihood of survival and reproduction. Not to be confused with functional specialization, which examines the specific parts of the brain that are engaged during specific behaviors or processes,[2] cognitive specialization is focused on characteristics of the mind (an internal entity), which in turn affects external behaviors. Most of these specializations are thought to have developed in areas of the neocortex unique to humans.[3] The most significant cognitive specializations among humans include theory of mind and language acquisition and production, while non-human animals may specialize in foraging behavior,[4] self-awareness, or other adaptive abilities.

Social behavior

Social communication is critical to effective human interaction, and has evolved over time to support the complex exchange of ideas.[5] [6] Some social behaviors, such as helping and altruism, are largely unique to humans and are instrumental in ensuring the survival of the species.[7] Evolutionary psychologists Leda Cosmides and John Tooby argue that the human mind contains "specialized mechanisms" that were designed by natural selection to facilitate social communication and exchange.[8] Without this specialized "algorithm", Cosmides and Tooby claim, social exchange among humans would be closer to that of our closest evolutionary ancestors, the great apes. In addition to humans' broad abilities supporting positive social interaction, Stone et al. (2002) put forth evidence for more specific specializations including "cheater detection" and "precautionary reasoning," both of which appear to serve strong adaptive purposes by allowing humans to share resources with only those who are likely to share with them in the future, and avoid sharing resources with untrustworthy individuals.[9] Overall, the adaptiveness of social communication has been examined in children, adults, and older adults,[10] across cultures,[11] and in neuropsychiatric populations.

Evidence for universality

If social behavior is to be considered a cognitive specialization unique to human neural architecture, it should be present in every human society. To provide cross-cultural evidence that cognitive adaptations specifically support social communication, Sugiyama, Tooby, and Cosmides investigated social reasoning in a tribe in the Ecuadorian Amazon. The Shiwiar, who are a hunter-horticulturalist group previously unexposed to the presented psychological stimuli, were "as highly proficient" in determining who cheated in a given situation as their counterparts in the United States. This performance indicates that social communication, at least in the domain of cheater detection, is not determined by one's culture. According to Sugiyama, Tooby, and Cosimdes, the social "algorithms" discussed above are present in both Western and non-Western populations, providing strong evidence for the universality of such a skill.

Theory of mind

Theory of mind, or the ability to attribute mental states to other people, is thought to be a cognitive specialization unique to humans, with a few possible exceptions discussed below.[12] [13] Theory of mind is thought to be critical in social cognition and communication because it allows us to distinguish between accidental and purposeful actions, to make judgments about others' internal states, and to determine how another's thoughts may differ from our own. The acquisition of theory of mind in humans mostly takes place during early childhood, and is thought to be fully developed by the early school years.[14] Theory of mind research in chimpanzees by social psychologists David Premack and Guy Woodruff in 1978 brought it to the forefront of psychological inquiry, though true theory of mind is only thought to exist in humans.[15] This phenomenon has been analyzed in many fields, and it is thought to be among the most beneficial specializations for survival of the human species, due to its facilitation of cooperation and interpersonal relationships.[16]

In autism

Theory of mind appears to be lacking in children with autism spectrum disorders, and this deficit is thought to be a major contributor to frequent impairments in some areas of social understanding in people with autism.[17] The fact that a developmental delay in (or absence of) theory of mind can impair social functioning—a skill imperative in the survival of the human species—is argued to be evidence for theory of mind as an adaptive cognitive specialization. Understanding that others may be thinking different thoughts than I am (colloquially, "putting oneself in another person's shoes") allows humans to communicate effectively and to live in large social groups. This adaptability is what makes theory of mind a cognitive specialization, rather than just another byproduct of human evolution: humankind has unique and beneficial communication skills, and this is partially due to our ability to recognize that other people may not think or know the same things we do.

Language

Though some (including Bates et al.[18] [19]) have argued that language arose as a byproduct of the evolution of humans' general cognitive abilities, Steven Pinker argues that it is, on its own, an adaptive mechanism. Drawing on existing literature and theory, he proposes several types of evidence for this claim, including the universality and ontogeny of language. Pinker also uses the double dissociation between general intelligence and language to argue for language as a specific adaptation. Those who lose language capabilities due to traumatic brain injury or stroke but maintain many other cognitive abilities exemplify Pinker's idea that language and general cognition are not always perfectly overlapping in human behavior. Using language "multiplies the benefit of knowledge" in multiple domains, including technology, tool use, and intentions of ourselves and others.

Evolution

Arbib puts forth a hypothesis that mirror neurons in the primate brain were a precursor to language abilities in humans.[20] Without these neurons in Broca's area in humans (which is analogous to F5 in monkeys), Arbib claims, we could not have evolved a specialization for language—which is used to explain why non-human animals do not have linguistic capabilities. In addition, Meguerditchian and Vauclair have argued that our evolutionary ancestors' communicative gestures (such as threat gestures and "food begs" among baboons) established a foundation on which to build human language skills.[21] This behavior was selected for, built upon, and modified, leading to the capabilities humans have today. Early theories explained early language as an adaptive way to communicate during a hunt, but recent research has focused on ecological theories that incorporate social demands; or, as Flinn et al. put it, a "social arms race" against non-human primates.[22] As a behavior selected for over the long term, with many successful "intermediary stages," human language differs from all other social behaviors among chimpanzees, which are thought to be more gradual in their evolutionary development. Further evidence for language as a cognitive specialization includes Ferreira et al.'s finding that some parts of language (for instance, syntax) can be spared in amnesia, while other abilities (like memory retention) are drastically reduced.[23] This and similar dissociations support the theory that specific neural architecture, which has evolved over time, supports language function.

Universal Grammar

Linguist Noam Chomsky proposed a biological component of language, which he termed Universal Grammar.[24] According to Chomsky, an essential part of language processing is hard-wired into the human brain. This allows language to be produced with or without specific linguistic instruction (which is closely associated with the poverty of the stimulus argument). All humans—and only humans—have this biological trait, but building blocks of universal grammar have been reported in other species.[25] Jackendoff argues that Universal Grammar is itself a "pre-existing cognitive specialization":[26] rather than needing explicit instruction on how to speak their native language, or having vocabulary and syntactical rules of a specific language present in their brains from birth, children seem to be genetically pre-disposed to learn language. Complementary to the connection made between area F5 in macaques' brains, the theory of Universal Grammar allows for an evolutionary perspective on language use as a cognitive specialization. There is some controversy, however, on whether or not Universal Grammar can have evolved by standard Darwinian evolutionary principles, or must be explained using different mechanisms.[27]

Benefits

According to Nowak and Sigmund, language is essential to human life as we know it.[28] Without the ability to verbally communicate with members of our social group, there would be no reciprocity (that is, returning of favors), and no way to cooperate with one another for a greater good. Some have argued that unique aspects of human language have evolved for unexpectedly beneficial reasons, besides simply asking for help or sharing information about the world. Gossip, viewed by many [29] to be a superfluous aspect of human communication, may even serve an adaptive purpose. The spread of information about other people, even if it is malicious, may serve as an indicator of social intelligence and a way to deter illicit behaviors. Though gossip likely helps some humans and hinders others' social standing, it appears to be an overall benefit of the ability to produce verbal language. Without an overall specialization for language (including such sub-specializations as gossip), linguists argue, humans would not be able to share information efficiently and effectively.

Other possible specializations

Watson et al. provide support for a specific specialization in language-dependent humor.[30] Its adaptive value has both extrinsic and intrinsic components: humor facilitates social bonding if shared extrinsically, and provides pleasure if enjoyed in one's own mind. In addition, Johnson-Frey (2003) proposed a unique human specialization for tool use.[31] According to Johnson-Frey, humans' ability to use tools is based on complex cognitive mechanisms, not just advanced sensorimotor skills. Rather than it being considered a purely physical specialization based only in motor areas of the brain, Johnson-Frey argues that tool use should be classified as a cognitive phenomenon due to its foundation in cognition. On a more philosophical level, Boyer (2003) argues that "religious thought and behavior" is a specialization that originally developed as a by-product of brain function, and its adaptive purposes led to its continued evolution by natural selection.[32] Krueger et al. (2007) have argued that trust, which may form the foundation for helping and altruism and thus the basis of human social interaction, is also a cognitive specialization.[33]

Non-human specialization

In non-human primates

Humankind's closest ancestors, the great apes, have evolved a number of specialized behaviors: orangutans are specialists at climbing trees, while chimpanzees and gorillas have evolved to walk on their knuckles. However, in considering non-behavioral specializations, Penn et al. (2008) argue that the "profound continuity" Charles Darwin noted between human and non-human animals in the biological domain is matched by a "profound discontinuity" between human and non-human animal minds.[34] In contrast, in addition to cognitive-behavioral adaptations, it is possible that chimpanzees have acquired more socially advanced skills through natural selection, including self-recognition (indicated by chimpanzees' established ability to pass the "mirror test").[35] This task—in which a successful trial is simply one in which an animal recognizes itself in a mirror—is thought to be a basic building block of theory of mind development.[36] Rhesus monkeys have also been shown to realize when they remember certain events and items, which is considered to be an instrumental building block in the formation of social relationships, as one must remember who owes him favors, who he can trust, and who he should avoid in order to prosper in the community.[37]

In other animals

More recent evidence has shown that cognitive specialization is not just present in primates: domesticated dogs may show signs of understanding human behavior and communication, indicating a social-cognitive specialization that is argued to make them more likely to receive food, shelter and love from their human owners. Being receptive to human behavioral indicators and responding accordingly has allowed dogs to survive and thrive as a species.[38] Bottlenose dolphins and elephants have also been shown to pass the "mirror test" explained above. This indication of some elementary self-awareness provides more evidence for foundational theory of mind skills in organisms throughout the animal kingdom.[39] [40] Ants, bees, and other insects have also evolved behaviors consistent with various specializations, including advanced navigational skills and several basic social communication abilities.[41] Adaptive cognitive evolution has been examined in pigeons' ability to group objects (which is argued to support their processing of and adaptation to novel environments),[42] problem solving and "creative" tool modification among rooks,[43] and tool use in crows.[44]

See also

Further reading

External links

Notes and References

  1. Povinelli . D. . Preuss . T . 1995 . Theory of mind: evolutionary history of a cognitive specialization . Trends in Neurosciences . 18 . 9. 418–424 . 10.1016/0166-2236(95)93939-U . 7482808 . 35128639 .
  2. Mahon . B. Z. . Jessica F. Cantlon . Cantlon . J. F. . 2011 . The specialization of function: Cognitive and neural perspectives . Cognitive Neuropsychology . 28 . 3–4. 147–155 . 10.1080/02643294.2011.633504 . 22185234 . 3964597 .
  3. Pinker, S. (1994). The language instinct: The new science of language and mind (Vol. 7529). Penguin UK.
  4. Toth . A. Robinson . 2007 . Evo-devo and the evolution of social behavior . Trends in Genetics . 23 . 7. 334–341 . 10.1016/j.tig.2007.05.001 . 17509723 .
  5. 23814325 . 10.1002/mde.1287 . 27 . Cognitive Adaptations for n-person Exchange: The Evolutionary Roots of Organizational Behavior . 3693395 . 2006 . MDE Manage Decis Econ . 103–129 . Tooby . J . Cosmides . L . Price . ME. 2–3 .
  6. Levinson . S . 2006 . Cognition at the heart of human interaction . Discourse Studies . 8 . 1. 85–93 . 10.1177/1461445606059557 . 11858/00-001M-0000-0013-1A74-B . 145542956 . free .
  7. 21331228 . 10.4161/cib.3.6.12604 . 3 . Coevolution of cooperation, causal cognition and mindreading . 3038052 . 2010 . Commun Integr Biol . 522–4 . Barrett . HC . Cosmides . L . Tooby . J. 6 .
  8. Cosmides . L. . Tooby . J. . 1992 . Cognitive adaptations for social exchange. The adapted mind . PNAS . 99. 17. 163–228 . 10.1073/pnas.122352999 . 12177409 . 123291 . 2002PNAS...9911537S . free .
  9. Stone . V. E. . Cosmides . L. . Tooby . J. . Kroll . N. . Knight . R. T. . 2002 . Selective impairment of reasoning about social exchange in a patient with bilateral limbic system damage . Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences . 99 . 17. 11531–11536 . 10.1073/pnas.122352699 . 12177408 . 123290 . 2002PNAS...9911531S . free .
  10. Povinelli, D. J., Prince, C. G., & Preuss, T. M. (2005). Parent-Offspring Conflict and the Development of Social Understanding. The Innate Mind: Structure and Contents: Structure and Contents, 239.
  11. Sugiyama . L. S. . Tooby . J. . Cosmides . L. . 2002 . Cross-cultural evidence of cognitive adaptations for social exchange among the Shiwiar of Ecuadorian Amazonia . Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences . 99 . 17. 11537–11542 . 10.1073/pnas.122352999 . 12177409 . 123291 . 2002PNAS...9911537S . free .
  12. Heyes . C.M. . 1998 . Theory of mind in nonhuman primates . Behavioral and Brain Sciences . 21 . 1. 101–114 . 10.1017/s0140525x98000703 . 10097012 . 6469633 .
  13. Jellema . T. . Baker . C. I. . Wicker . B. . Perrett . D. I. . 2000 . Neural representation for the perception of the intentionality of actions . Brain and Cognition . 44 . 2. 280–302 . 10.1006/brcg.2000.1231 . 11041992 . 12580222 .
  14. Wellman . H. M. . Cross . D. . Watson . J. . 2001 . Meta‐analysis of theory‐of‐mind development: the truth about false belief . Child Development . 72 . 3. 655–684 . 11405571 . 10.1111/1467-8624.00304 . 11269306 .
  15. Premack . D. . Woodruff . G. . 1978 . Does the chimpanzee have a theory of mind? . Behav Sci . 4 . 515–526 .
  16. Pinker . S . 1999 . How the mind works . Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences . 882 . 1. 119–127 . 10.1111/j.1749-6632.1999.tb08538.x . 10415890 . 1999NYASA.882..119P . 222083447 .
  17. Baron-Cohen . Simon . Leslie . Alan M. . Frith . Uta . Does the autistic child have a "theory of mind"? . . 21 . 1 . 37 - 46 . . 10.1016/0010-0277(85)90022-8 . 2934210 . October 1985 . 14955234 . Pdf.
  18. Bates . E. . Thal . D. . Marchman . V. . 1991 . Symbols and syntax: A Darwinian approach to language development . Biological and Behavioral Determinants of Language Development . 29 . 66 .
  19. Pinker . S . 2003 . Language as an adaptation to the cognitive niche . Studies in the Evolution of Language . 3 . 16–37 . 10.1093/acprof:oso/9780199244843.003.0002 . 9780199244843 .
  20. Arbib . M. A. . 2005 . From monkey-like action recognition to human language: An evolutionary framework for neurolinguistics . Behavioral and Brain Sciences . 28 . 2. 105–124 . 16201457 . 10.1017/s0140525x05000038 . 2379278 .
  21. 19091390 . 10.1016/j.bandl.2008.10.004 . 108 . Contrast of hand preferences between communicative gestures and non-communicative actions in baboons: implications for the origins of hemispheric specialization for language . 2009 . Brain Lang . 167–74 . Meguerditchian . A . Vauclair . J. 3 . 14539576 .
  22. Flinn . M. V. . Geary . D. C. . Ward . C. V. . 2005 . Ecological dominance, social competition, and coalitionary arms races: Why humans evolved extraordinary intelligence . Evolution and Human Behavior . 26 . 1. 10–46 . 10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2004.08.005 .
  23. Ferreira . V. S. . Bock . K. . Wilson . M. P. . Cohen . N. J. . 2008 . Memory for syntax despite amnesia . Psychological Science . 19 . 9. 940–946 . 18947361 . 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2008.02180.x . 2659624 .
  24. Chomsky, N. (1972). Language and mind (p. 100). New York: Harcourt Brace Jovanovich.
  25. Hauser . Marc D. . Chomsky . Noam . Fitch . W. Tecumseh . 2002 . The Faculty of Language: What Is It, Who Has It, and How Did It Evolve? . Science . 298 . 5598. 1569–1579 . 10.1126/science.298.5598.1569 . 12446899 .
  26. Jackendoff, R. (2001). Language in the ecology of mind. The Routledge companion to semiotics and linguistics, 52–65.
  27. Hinzen . Wolfram . 2012 . The philosophical significance of Universal Grammar . Language Sciences . 34 . 5. 635–649 . 10.1016/j.langsci.2012.03.005 .
  28. Nowak . M. A. . Sigmund . K. . 2005 . Evolution of indirect reciprocity . Nature . 437 . 7063. 1291–1298 . 16251955 . 10.1038/nature04131 . 2005Natur.437.1291N . 3153895 .
  29. 10.1525/aa.2001.103.4.1196 . 103 . The Mating Mind: How Sexual Choice Shaped the Evolution of the Human Mind:The Mating Mind: How Sexual Choice Shaped the Evolution of the Human Mind. . 2001 . American Anthropologist . 1196–1198 . Kurland . Jeffrey A.. 4 .
  30. Watson . K. K. . Matthews . B. J. . Allman . J. M. . 2007 . Brain activation during sight gags and language-dependent humor . Cerebral Cortex . 17 . 2. 314–324 . 16514105 . 10.1093/cercor/bhj149 . free .
  31. Johnson-Frey . S. H. . 2003 . What's so special about human tool use? . Neuron . 39 . 2. 201–204 . 10.1016/s0896-6273(03)00424-0 . 12873378 . 18437970 . free .
  32. 12639693 . 7 . Religious thought and behaviour as by-products of brain function . 2003 . Trends Cogn Sci . 119–124 . Boyer . P . 3 . 10.1016/s1364-6613(03)00031-7. 1935122 .
  33. Krueger . F. . McCabe . K. . Moll . J. . Kriegeskorte . N. . Zahn . R. . Strenziok . M. . Grafman . J. . 2007 . Neural correlates of trust . Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences . 104 . 50. 20084–20089 . 10.1073/pnas.0710103104 . 18056800 . 2148426 . 2007PNAS..10420084K . free .
  34. Penn . D. C. . Holyoak . K. J. . Povinelli . D. J. . 2008 . Darwin's mistake: Explaining the discontinuity between human and nonhuman minds . Behavioral and Brain Sciences . 31 . 2. 109–130 . 18479531 . 10.1017/S0140525X08003543 . 218218 .
  35. Heyes . C. M. . 1998 . Theory of mind in nonhuman primates . Behavioral and Brain Sciences . 21 . 1. 101–114 . 10097012 . 10.1017/s0140525x98000703 . 6469633 .
  36. Povinelli . D. J. . Eddy . T. J. . Hobson . R. P. . Tomasello . M. . 1996 . What young chimpanzees know about seeing . Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development . 61. 3. i-vi, 1–152; discussion 153–91. 10.2307/1166159 . 1166159 . 8795292 .
  37. Hampton . R. R. . 2001 . Rhesus monkeys know when they remember . Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences . 98 . 9. 5359–5362 . 10.1073/pnas.071600998 . 11274360 . 33214 . 2001PNAS...98.5359H . free .
  38. Hare . B. . Tomasello . M. . 2005 . Human-like social skills in dogs? . Trends in Cognitive Sciences . 9 . 9. 439–444 . 10.1016/j.tics.2005.07.003 . 16061417 . 9311402 .
  39. 8521259 . 10.1006/ccog.1995.1026 . 4 . Using self-view television to distinguish between self-examination and social behavior in the bottlenose dolphin (Tursiops truncatus) . 1995 . Conscious Cogn . 205–24 . Marten . K . Psarakos . S. 2 . 44372881 .
  40. Plotnik . J.M. . de Waal . F.B.M. . Reiss . D. . 2006 . Self-recognition in an Asian elephant . Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences . 103 . 45. 17053–17057 . 1636577. 10.1073/pnas.0608062103 . 19514017 . free .
  41. 17509723 . 10.1016/j.tig.2007.05.001 . 23 . Evo-devo and the evolution of social behavior . 2007 . Trends Genet . 334–41 . Toth . AL . Robinson . GE. 7 .
  42. Kirsch . J. A. . Kabanova . A. . Güntürkün . O. . 2008 . Grouping of artificial objects in pigeons: an inquiry into the cognitive architecture of an avian mind . Brain Research Bulletin . 75 . 2. 485–490 . 10.1016/j.brainresbull.2007.10.033 . 18331919 . 12759880 .
  43. Bird . C. D. . Emery . N. J. . 2009 . Insightful problem solving and creative tool modification by captive nontool-using rooks . Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences . 106 . 25. 10370–10375 . 10.1073/pnas.0901008106 . 19478068 . 2700937 . free .
  44. Hunt . G. R. . 2000 . Human–like, population–level specialization in the manufacture of pandanus tools by New Caledonian crows Corvus moneduloides . Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences . 267 . 1441. 403–413 . 10.1098/rspb.2000.1015 . 10722223 . 1690543 .