Central place foraging (CPF) theory is an evolutionary ecology model for analyzing how an organism can maximize foraging rates while traveling through a patch (a discrete resource concentration), but maintains the key distinction of a forager traveling from a home base to a distant foraging location rather than simply passing through an area or travelling at random. CPF was initially developed to explain how red-winged blackbirds might maximize energy returns when traveling to and from a nest. The model has been further refined and used by anthropologists studying human behavioral ecology and archaeology.
Orians and Pearson (1979) found that red-winged blackbirds in eastern Washington State tend to capture a larger number of single species prey items per trip compared to the same species in Costa Rica, which brought back large, single insects. Foraging specialization by Costa Rican blackbirds was attributed to increased search and handling costs of nocturnal foraging, whereas birds in Eastern Washington forage diurnally for prey with lower search and handling costs. Studies with sea birds and seals have also found that load size tends to increase with foraging distance from the nest, as predicted by CPF. Other central place foragers, such as social insects, also show support for CPF theory. European honeybees increase their nectar load as travel time to nectar sites from a hive increases. Beavers have been found to preferentially collect larger diameter trees as distance from their lodge increases.
To apply the central place foraging model to ethnographic and experimental archaeological data driven by middle range theory, Bettinger et al. (1997) simplify the Barlow and Metcalf (1996) central place model to explore the archaeological implications of acorn (Quercus kelloggii) and mussel (Mytilus californianus) procurement and processing. This model assumes foragers are gathering resources at a distance from their central place with the goal of efficiently returning the resource home. Travel time is expected to determine the degree to which foragers will process a resource in order to increase its utility prior to returning from a foraging location to their central place.Transport capabilities in aboriginal California were established by measuring the volume of burden baskets and extrapolating the load weight based on ethnographic data on basket use.
Ethnographic and experimental data was used to estimate utility at each possible stage of processing.Examining ecology and procurement methods, the central place foraging model was used to predict the conditions in which field processing of the two species will occur.
An understanding of central place foraging has implications for studying archaeological site formation. Variability of remains at sites can tell us about mobility – whether or not groups are central place foragers, what resource they’re mapping on to, and their degree of mobility. Based on central place foraging application for the processing of mussels and acorns, Bettinger et al. (1997) make several predictions for archaeological expectations. The study shows that procurement with field processing is more costly compared to foraging and processing resources residentially. These results imply that highly mobile foragers will establish a home base in close proximity to staple resources, and all processing of those resources will be done residentially. Less residentially mobile populations would in turn be mapped only onto a few resources, and would be expected to field process non-local resources on logistical procurement forays at greater distances from their central place. Processing debris from archaeological sites should reflect changes in mobility.
Glover (2009) used a CPF model to determine if late nineteenth century silver miners near Gothic, Colorado were choosing mine locations efficiently given the costs of transporting silver ore to the mill, the value of silver, and the amount of silver per kilogram of ore. Estimates of the costs associated with transport were obtained using research from physiology to determine the most energetically efficient load size. Newspaper articles were used to determine the hourly wage that a miner could be making if they worked in town instead. Newspapers were also used to estimate the value of silver at that time, and estimates of the amount of silver per kilogram of ore were obtained through records from area silver mills, as well as through newspapers. These differed, with the newspapers optimistically claiming that silver deposits were far more productive than the more accurate mill records demonstrated.
These estimates were used to determine the optimal placement of mines. A number of historic mining locations were recorded using GPS. These data were used to calculate least cost paths from the mines to Gothic, which provided the distances to the central place. The results were compared to two different CPF models based on newspaper propaganda and the more realistic mill records, respectively.
Miners were choosing locations that were much farther away than feasible given the value of silver and its actual abundance. However, the mines were within the distance predicted using the optimistic newspaper estimates. Glover suggested that miners, being new to the area, used social learning strategies and based their decisions on newspaper propaganda and rumors, rather than individual experience. Therefore, they chose locations that were too far away to be economically viable.
Shellfish exemplify the resources targeted by the CPF model – those with a heavy, bulky,low utility component (e.g. shell) surrounding a smaller, lighter high utility component(e.g. meat). If foragers differentially field process and transport shellfish prey items,analyses of midden composition may incorrectly estimate the importance of some speciesand their relative contribution to prehistoric diets. Using foraging data from the Meriamof Australia, Bird and Bliege Bird (1997) compare observed shellfish field acquisition toshell deposition at residential sites, and test the hypotheses of the CPF model.
The Meriam inhabit Torres Strait Islands of Australia, are of Melanesian descent, andhave strong cultural and historical ties to New Guinea. They continue to harvest marineresources such as sea turtles, fishes, squid, and shellfish. Bird and Bliege Bird conducted“focal individual foraging follows” of 33 children, 16 men and 42 women duringintertidal foraging bouts on reef flats and rock shores. Foraging technology includes 10-liter plastic buckets, long-blade knives, and hammers. Foragers are constrained by time(2–4 hours at low tide) and load size (10-liter bucket).
Large clams (Hippopus hippopus and Tridacna spp.) collected on the reef flat constitute over half of the edible weight collected, but since they are almost always field processed their shells make up only 10% of the residential site deposition. In contrast, sunset clams (Asaphis violascens) and nerites (Nerita undata) are usually processed residentially. Large clams were, therefore, underrepresented while small clams and nerites were overrepresented in the reconstructed diet.
Since reef flat and rocky shore foraging occurs at multiple sites at variable distances from the residential camp, the authors calculated the mean one-way travel distance processing threshold (
zd1
zd1
For gastropods (Lambis lambis,
zd1
Overall, prey types that were difficult or inefficient to process and/or were collected near the residential or temporary camp were not field processed. Species that required little processing time to increase returns and/or were collected far from camp were field processed. The field processing predictions of the CPF model might be incorrect where shellfish are transported whole in order to maintain freshness for later consumption or trade, or where the shell itself is valuable.
Barlow and Metcalfe (1996) address the issues of field processing of plant materials. Decisionsof central place foragers may confound archaeological interpretations about the contribution plantmaterial to the diet. Two interrelated issues are pertinent: the location of the central place, and fieldprocessing.
Barlow and Metcalfe study archaeological materials from two sites, Danger Cave and Hogup Cave, in thearea of the Great Salt Lake. These sites contain evidence for the use of piñon pine(Pinus monophylla) and pickleweed (Allenrolfea occidentalis).
Samples were obtained for experimental processing from extant piñon groves and pickleweed patchesin the vicinity as the cave sites. Piñon and pickleweed were harvested and processed in carefully timedand controlled stages. After each stage the useful, i.e. edible, portion of the remaining material wasweighed and recorded before proceeding to the next stage. Stages consisted of: gathering, drying, anda variety of processes (parching, hulling, winnowing, etc.) to remove inedible constituents. Caloricvalues of the samples were then determined via laboratory analysis. These values, as well as assumedload sizes from 3 to 15 kg (based on ethnographic burden basket sizes) were then used to generate fieldprocessing model predictions.
At a distance of 15 kilometers from the central place, the estimated net return rates for field processing loads of piñon and pickleweed are 3,000 and 190 calories per hour, respectively. Since piñon has higher overall return rates, field processing produces a higher rate of return. Because pickleweed has a lower rate of return, it is not worthwhile to spend the additional effort required for field processing.Therefore, the central place will be situated closer to pickleweed patches than to piñon in order to more effectively exploit the lower-ranked resource.
These results imply that the archaeological evidence for pickleweed at the cave may over estimate its actual contribution to the diet. If foragers choose to reside closer to pickleweed patches and bring back largely unprocessed plants, a high density of pickleweed macrofossils will be incorporated into site deposits. However, the opposite is true for piñon, which is largely processed in the field. Thus, most sites will contain little macrofossil evidence of the inedible portions of piñon that could later be recovered by archaeologists. As such, the relative abundance of macrofossils in most cases does not directly translate into the relative contribution of those resources to the diet of central place foragers.
The goal of the field processing model is for a forager to maximize its return rate per roundtrip from home base to patch. The model typically solves for some amount of travel time that makes it worthwhile to process a resource to a certain stage. To determine this, we need to relate the benefit of processing and the time spent processing to the travel time. We let
z=
x0=
x1=
y0=
y1=
The relationship is then specified by:
z= | y0x1-y1x0 |
y1-y0 |
With values for the utility and time of processed
(y1,x1)
(y0,x0)
z
If
x1>x0
y1>y0
If
y0<y1
y0 | \geq | |
x0 |
y1 | |
x1 |
Many resources have multiple components that can be removed during processing to increase utility. Multistage field processing models provide a way to calculate travel thresholds for each stage when a resource has more than one component. As one increases the utility per load, the time needed to procure a complete load increases.
The benefit of each stage of processing is:
y | ||||||||||||||||
|
where
Aj=
Bj=
yj=
The cost in terms of time for each stage of processing is:
x | ||||||||||
|
\right)\left(M+\sum | |
i\notinsj |
Di\right)
where
Dj=
L=
P=
M=
xj=
Now these values can be used to calculate
zj
This model rests on a number of assumptions. The most important are listed here.
There are three key predictions from the field processing model.
x0
x1
x1
x1
z
y1
y0
x0
z
Transport decay curves demonstrate the reduction in return rates (cal/hour) experienced by a central place forager as a function of round trip travel time.