Two-alternative forced choice (2AFC) is a method for measuring the sensitivity of a person or animal to some particular sensory input, stimulus, through that observer's pattern of choices and response times to two versions of the sensory input. For example, to determine a person's sensitivity to dim light, the observer would be presented with a series of trials in which a dim light was randomly either in the top or bottom of the display. After each trial, the observer responds "top" or "bottom". The observer is not allowed to say "I do not know", or "I am not sure", or "I did not see anything". In that sense the observer's choice is forced between the two alternatives.
Both options can be presented concurrently (as in the above example) or sequentially in two intervals (also known as two-interval forced choice, 2IFC). For example, to determine sensitivity to a dim light in a two-interval forced choice procedure, an observer could be presented with series of trials comprising two sub-trials (intervals) in which the dim light is presented randomly in the first or the second interval. After each trial, the observer responds only "first" or "second".
The term 2AFC is sometimes used to describe a task in which an observer is presented with a single stimulus and must choose between one of two alternatives. For example in a lexical decision task a participant observes a string of characters and must respond whether the string is a "word" or "non-word". Another example is the random dot kinetogram task, in which a participant must decide whether a group of moving dots are predominately moving "left" or "right". The results of these tasks, sometimes called yes-no tasks, are much more likely to be affected by various response biases than 2AFC tasks. For example, with extremely dim lights, a person might respond, completely truthfully, "no" (i.e., "I did not see any light") on every trial, whereas the results of a 2AFC task will show the person can reliably determine the location (top or bottom) of the same, extremely dim light.
2AFC is a method of psychophysics developed by Gustav Theodor Fechner.[1]
There are various manipulations in the design of the task, engineered to test specific behavioral dynamics of choice. In one well known experiment of attention that examines the attentional shift, the Posner Cueing Task uses a 2AFC design to present two stimuli representing two given locations.[2] In this design there is an arrow that cues which stimulus (location) to attend to. The person then has to make a response between the two stimuli (locations) when prompted. In animals, the 2AFC task has been used to test reinforcement probability learning, for example such as choices in pigeons after reinforcement of trials.[3] A 2AFC task has also been designed to test decision making and the interaction of reward and probability learning in monkeys.[4] Monkeys were trained to look at a center stimulus and were then presented with two salient stimuli side by side. A response can then be made in the form of a saccade to the left or to the right stimulus. A juice reward is then administered after each response. The amount of juice reward is then varied to modulate choice.
In a different application, the 2AFC is designed to test discrimination of motion perception. The random dot motion coherence task, introduces a random dot kinetogram, with a percentage of net coherent motion distributed across the random dots.[5] [6] The percentage of dots moving together in a given direction determines the coherence of motion towards the direction. In most experiments, the participant must make a choice response between two directions of motion (e.g. up or down), usually indicated by a motor response such as a saccade or pressing a button.
It is possible to introduce biases in decision making in the 2AFC task. For example, if one stimulus occurs with more frequency than the other, then the frequency of exposure to the stimuli may influence the participant's beliefs about the probability of the occurrence of the alternatives.[7] Introducing biases in the 2AFC task is used to modulate decision making and examine the underlying processes.
The 2AFC task has yielded consistent behavioral results on decision-making, which lead to the development of theoretical and computational models of the dynamics and results of decision-making.[8] [9] [10] [11] [12] [13] [14] [15] [16]
Suppose the two stimuli
x1
x2
a
b
N(\mua,\sigmaa)
N(\mub,\sigmab)
x1,x2
a
b
b
a
The probability of error with this ideal decision strategy is given by the generalized chi-square distribution:
2 | |
p(e)=p\left(\tilde{\chi} | |
\boldsymbol{w |
,\boldsymbol{k},\boldsymbol{λ},0,0}\right)<0
\boldsymbol{w}=\begin{bmatrix}
2 | |
\sigma | |
a |
&
2 | |
-\sigma | |
b |
\end{bmatrix}, \boldsymbol{k}=\begin{bmatrix}1&1\end{bmatrix}, \boldsymbol{λ}=
\mua-\mub | ||||||
|
\begin{bmatrix}
2 | |
\sigma | |
a |
&
2 | |
\sigma | |
b |
\end{bmatrix}.
This model can also extend to the cases when each of the two stimuli is itself a multivariate normal vector, and also to the situations when the two categories have different prior probabilities, or the decisions are biased due to different values attached to the possible outcomes.
There are typically three assumptions made by computational models using the 2AFC:
It is typically assumed that the difference in evidence favoring each alternative is the quantity tracked over time and that which ultimately informs the decision; however, evidence for different alternatives could be tracked separately.
The drift-diffusion model (DDM) is a well defined[18] model, that is proposed to implement an optimal decision policy for 2AFC.[19] It is the continuous analog of a random walk model.The DDM assumes that in a 2AFC task, the subject is accumulating evidence for one or other of the alternatives at each time step, and integrating that evidence until a decision threshold is reached. As the sensory input which constitutes the evidence is noisy, the accumulation to the threshold is stochastic rather than deterministic – this gives rise to the directed random-walk-like behavior.The DDM has been shown to describe accuracy and reaction times in human data for 2AFC tasks.
The accumulation of evidence in the DDM is governed according to the following formula:
dx=Adt+cdW , x(0)=0
At time zero, the evidence accumulated, x, is set equal to zero. At each time step, some evidence, A, is accumulated for one of the two possibilities in the 2AFC. A is positive if the correct response is represented by the upper threshold, and negative if the lower. In addition, a noise term, cdW, is added to represent noise in input. On average, the noise will integrate to zero. The extended DDM allows for selection of
A
x(0)
The Ornstein–Uhlenbeck model extends the DDM by adding another term,
λ
dx = (λx+A)dt + cdW
In the race model,[22] [23] evidence for each alternative is accumulated separately, and a decision made either when one of the accumulators reaches a predetermined threshold, or when a decision is forced and then the decision associated with the accumulator with the highest evidence is chosen. This can be represented formally by:
\begin{align}dy1 = I1dt + cdW1\\ dy2 = I2dt + cdW2\end{align}, y1(0) = y2(0)=0
The race model is not mathematically reducible to the DDM, and hence cannot be used to implement an optimal decision procedure.
The mutual inhibition model also uses two accumulators to model the accumulation of evidence, as with the race model. In this model the two accumulators have an inhibitory effect on each other, so as evidence is accumulated in one, it dampens the accumulation of evidence in the other. In addition, leaky accumulators are used, so that over time evidence accumulated decays – this helps to prevent runaway accumulation towards one alternative based on a short run of evidence in one direction. Formally, this can be shown as:
\begin{align}dy1 = (-ky1-wy2+I1)dt + cdW1\\ dy2 = (-ky2-wy1+I2)dt + cdW2\end{align}, y1(0) = y2(0)=0
Where
k
w
The feedforward inhibition model is similar to the mutual inhibition model, but instead of being inhibited by the current value of the other accumulator, each accumulator is inhibited by a fraction of the input to the other. It can be formally stated thus:
\begin{align}dy1 = I1dt + cdW1 - u(I2dt + cdW2)\\ dy2 = I2dt + cdW2 - u(I1dt + cdW1)\end{align}, y1(0) = y2(0)=0
Where
u
Wang[24] suggested the pooled inhibition model, where a third, decaying accumulator is driven by accumulation in both of the accumulators used for decision making, and in addition to the decay used in the mutual inhibition model, each of the decision driving accumulators self-reinforce based on their current value. It can be formally stated thus:
\begin{align}dy1 = (-ky1-wy3+vy1+I1)dt + cdW1\\ dy2 = (-ky2-wy3+vy2+I2)dt + cdW2\ dy3 = (-kinhy3+w'(y1+y2))dt\end{align}
The third accumulator has an independent decay coefficient,
kinh
w'
In the parietal lobe, lateral intraparietal cortex (LIP) neuron firing rate in monkeys predicted the choice response of direction of motion suggesting this area is involved in decision making in the 2AFC.[25] [26]
Neural data recorded from LIP neurons in rhesus monkeys supports the DDM, as firing rates for the direction selective neuronal populations sensitive to the two directions used in the 2AFC task increase firing rates at stimulus onset, and average activity in the neuronal populations is biased in the direction of the correct response.[27] [28] [29] In addition, it appears that a fixed threshold of neuronal spiking rate is used as the decision boundary for each 2AFC task.[30]