ǂʼAmkoe language should not be confused with Western ǂHoan.
ǂʼAmkoe | |
Also Known As: | Formerly ǂHoan |
Region: | Botswana |
Speakers: | 20–50 Western ǂʼAmkoe |
Date: | 2015 |
Speakers2: | unknown number Eastern ǂʼAmkoe |
Ref: | [1] |
Familycolor: | Khoisan |
Fam1: | Kxʼa |
Dia1: | Nǃaqriaxe |
Dia2: | (Eastern) ǂHoan |
Dia3: | Sasi |
Iso3: | huc |
Notice: | IPA |
Glotto: | hoaa1235 |
Glottorefname: | Amkoe |
ǂʼAmkoe, formerly called by the dialectal name ǂHoan (Eastern ǂHȍã, ǂHûân, ǂHua, ǂHû, or in native orthography ǂHȍȁn), is a severely endangered Kxʼa language of Botswana. West ǂʼAmkoe dialect, along with Taa (or perhaps the Tsaasi dialect of Taa) and Gǀui, form the core of the Kalahari Basin sprachbund, and share a number of characteristic features, including the largest consonant inventories in the world. ǂʼAmkoe was shown to be related to the Juu languages by Honken and Heine (2010), and these have since been classified together in the Kxʼa language family.[2]
ǂʼAmkoe is moribund and severely endangered. There are only a few dozen native speakers, most born before 1960 (one Sasi speaker was born in 1971, one Nǃaqriaxe speaker in 1969), many of whom no longer speak the language fluently. The first language of the younger generations, and even of many older, native speakers who no longer speak ǂʼAmkoe well, is Gǀui, a Khoe language, in the case of Nǃaqriaxe; Kgalagadi, a Bantu language that is the local lingua franca, in the case of ǂHoan; and the Ngwato dialect of Tswana, in the case of Sasi.
ǂʼAmkoe (pronounced as /huc/; pronounced with the palatal nasal click, pronounced as /link/) is spoken in three areas in southeastern Botswana, corresponding to three dialects. Recent surveys found the following locations:
Nǃaqriaxe and ǂHoan are closest, collectively referred to as West ǂʼAmkoe; Sasi is referred to as East ǂʼAmkoe.
There are some phonological differences between the Nǃaqriaxe spoken around Dutlwe and that spoken around Motokwe and Khekhenye. Sasi is a "mutually intelligible language" with differences in phonology and lexicon.[3] There have been no systematic studies of Sasi, in 2015, Collins was engaged in fieldwork. The East and West populations had no knowledge of each other, but when brought together in 1996, they were able to communicate, and found the differences amusing.[4]
Recent scholars such as Collins, Gruber, Köhler, and Güldemann restrict the name ǂHoan to the ǂHoan dialect, and call the language as a whole ǂʼAmkoe, which means "person" in all dialects. Almost all linguistic work has been on the ǂHoan and Nǃaqriaxe dialects.
ǂHoan has gone by the names and spellings Eastern ǂHoan, ǂHùã, ǂHũa, ǂHṍã, ǂHoang de Dutlwe. It has been specified as Eastern ǂHoan to distinguish it from Western ǂHuan, a dialect of the unrelated Taa language.[5] Sasi has gone by Sàsí, Tshasi, Tshasi de Khutse. Tswana: Tshasi is a Tswana name that is more precise than the generic Masarwa "Bushman". The disambiguator de Khutse is used to distinguish it from a variety of Taa also called Tshase and Sase. The name of the third dialect is nǃàqrīāχè (pronounced as /[ǃ̃àˤɾīāχè]/) or àqrīāχè (pronounced as /[ʔàˤɾīāχè]/) in ǂʼAmkoe.
All Nǃaqriaxe speakers are bilingual in Gǀui, with some Kgalagadi as well. ǂHoan speakers are bilingual in Kgalagadi, and Sasi speakers in the Ngwato dialect of Tswana. The ǂʼAmkoe language shows evidence that it previously had extensive contact with Taa. Superimposed on this are Gǀui features such as a shift of alveolar consonants to palatal, even in ǂHoan, which is not currently in contact with Gǀui.
ǂʼAmkoe has bilabial clicks, which are found in only two other living languages.[6] It has been in intense contact with Gǀui and previously with Taa, and some of the sounds of ǂʼAmkoe appear to have been borrowed from Gǀui. On the other hand, the moribund state of the language is apparent in its phonology, and sounds not found in Gǀui appear to have been lost by many of the remaining speakers.
Nǃaqriaxe vowel qualities are pronounced as //i e a o u//. The front vowels, pronounced as //i e//, are very similar in formant space, as are even more so the back vowels, pronounced as //o u//, but minimal pairs distinguish them. Vowels may be nasalized, pharyngealized (written with a final in the practical orthography), or glottalized. Gerlach (2015) treats long vowels as sequences, in which the nasalized vowels, pronounced as //ĩ ã ũ//, occur phonemically only as V2, while the pharyngealized and glottalized vowels, pronounced as //aˤ oˤ// and pronounced as //aˀ oˀ uˀ// (and, in one loan word, pronounced as //iˀ//) occur only as V1. A vowel at V1 will be phonetically nasalized if V2 is nasal, though combinations of glottalized or pharyngealized plus nasalized vowels are not common. Some speakers glottalize pharyngeal vowels, but inconsistently, and it does not appear to be distinctive. Breathy vowels occur after aspirated consonants and with some speakers at the ends of utterances. Neither case is phonemic. They may also occur on some words with low tone. Not all words with low tone are attested with breathy vowels, but the feature does not appear to be distinctive (there are no minimal pairs), and so Gerlach (2015) does not treat breathy vowels as phonemic. pronounced as //o// is a diphthong pronounced as /[oa]/ before final pronounced as //m// (that is, in words of the shape Com), but it carries only a single tone, and so is analyzed as an allophone of a single pronounced as //o// vowel. This diphthongization occurs in all three dialects, and also in Gǀui, which probably got it from ǂʼAmkoe.
Honken (2013), which is based on Gruber (1973), says the ǂHȍã qualities, also pronounced as //a e i o u//, may be modal, breathy, laryngealized, or pharyngealized, and that all may be nasalized.
In words of the shape CVV, attested vowel sequences (considering only vowel quality) are aa, ee, ii, oo, uu, ai, ui, eo, oa, ua. Basically, vowel one is normally /a/ or /o/; an /o/ becomes /u/ before a high vowel two (such as /i/), while an /a/ becomes /e/ or /i/ consonant one is dental/palatal or if vowel two is high. These patterns may be an influence of ǀGui (Honken 2013).
Tone in ǂʼAmkoe shows acoustic characteristics of both neighbouring consonants and vowel phonation. Gerlach (2015) analyzes Nǃaqriaxe as having three phonemic tones: low (L), mid (M), and high (H). Monosyllabic words with CVV, CVN shapes have two tones. In disyllabic words of CV.CV shape, six tone combinations are found: a word may have a mid tone or go up or down between adjacent tones, but L.H and H.L are not attested, and the only falling tone is ML. All syllable-final tones in Gerlach's data are falling, perhaps an effect of utterance-final prosody. The system is very similar to that of Gǀui, Gerlach's analysis was based on data from a single speaker who speaks more Gǀui than ǂʼAmkoe and who lacks ǂʼAmkoe consonants not found in Gǀui, so it's not clear that the results are representative of ǂʼAmkoe as a whole.
The tones analyzed as rising phonemically are phonetically dipping (falling-rising). Voiced and aspirated consonants are tone depressors, with high tone at the level of mid after a tenuis or glottalized consonant, and mid at the level of low. (However, the endpoint of low>mid tone does not change, and so effectively becomes low>high.) Aspirated consonants (and especially delayed-aspirated clicks) have an additional depressive effect at the beginning of the tone, so that they are phonetically rising; the contour, however, is a sharp rise at the beginning, rather than the slow descent with a sharp rise at the end of the phonemically rising tones.
Collins (2012) describes six word tones for ǂHȍã dialect: extra high, high-mid (high level), mid-ow (mid level), high-low, low-mid, and low level. The extra high tones mostly occurs on high vowels, pronounced as //i u//, which have an allophonic pitch-raising effect, whereas low level occurs after voiced consonants, which have a tone depressor effect. Given that Collins did not control for initial consonants in his analysis, his description is consistent with Gerlach's for Nǃaqriaxe dialect.
Gerlach (2012)[7] reports different consonant inventories for different speakers of Nǃaqriaxe dialect: A smaller one, similar to that of the neighboring Gǀui language and to previous accounts, is used by most speakers, including those who speak more Gǀui than ǂʼAmkoe. A larger inventory is believed to be more conservative, with pre-voiced consonants cognate to those of the related Ju languages and therefore perhaps dating back to proto-Kxʼa, but lost under Gǀui influence as the language became moribund. (These additional consonants are shaded in the table below.) Similar consonants are found in the neighboring Taa language; it is not clear if they date to proto-Tuu, and perhaps an earlier era of contact, or if Taa might have gotten them from ǂʼAmkoe.
The egressive consonants found in word-initial (C1) position in lexical words are as follows. Those in parentheses are only found in loan words. Those with a shaded background are only used by speakers Gerlach (2015) believes are conservative:
Alveolar | Post- alveolar | Palatal | Velar | Uvular | Glottal | ||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Nasal | (pronounced as /ink/) | (pronounced as /ink/) | pronounced as /ink/ (rare) | ||||||||
Plosive | pronounced as /ink/ (rare) | (pronounced as /ink/) | pronounced as /ink/ | pronounced as /ink/ | pronounced as /ink/ | pronounced as /ink/ | pronounced as /ink/ (rare) | ||||
(pronounced as /ink/) | (pronounced as /ink/) | pronounced as /ink/ | pronounced as /ink/ | pronounced as /ink/ | pronounced as /ink/ | pronounced as /ink/ | pronounced as /ink/ (epenthetic?) | ||||
(pronounced as /link/) | (pronounced as /link/) | pronounced as /link/ | pronounced as /link/ | pronounced as /link/ | pronounced as /link/ | pronounced as /link/ | |||||
pronounced as /link/ | |||||||||||
pronounced as /ink/ | pronounced as /ink/ | pronounced as /ink/ (rare) | pronounced as /ink/ (rare) | pronounced as /ink/ | |||||||
pronounced as /link/ | pronounced as /link/ | pronounced as /link/ | |||||||||
(pronounced as /link/)? | (pronounced as /link/)? | (pronounced as /link/)? | |||||||||
pronounced as /link/ | pronounced as /link/ | pronounced as /link/ | |||||||||
pronounced as /link/ | |||||||||||
Fricative | pronounced as /ink/ | (pronounced as /ink/)? | (pronounced as /ink/) | ||||||||
Approximant | (pronounced as /ink/) | (pronounced as /ink/) (pronounced as /ink/) |
The shaded consonants have a voiced hold and voiceless release, pronounced as /[dsʰ, dsʼ, ɡkʼ, ɢχʼ, dsqχʼ]/. Gerlach (2015) analyses the change of voicing as being a phonetic detail due to the nature of the release rather than being phonemically prevoiced. The post-alveolar affricates (pronounced as //tʃ// etc.) are conflated with the alveolar affricates (pronounced as //ts// etc.) in Nǃaqriaxe dialect. They are probably an old distinction that's been lost in Nǃaqriaxe. Where they do occur, they may be alveolo-palatal (pronounced as /[tɕ]/, etc.), depending on speaker and location. pronounced as //χ, tsχ, cχ// appear to be only found in Gǀui loans. Sasi has pronounced as //qʼ// rather than the pronounced as //qχʼ// of ǂHoan and Nǃaqriaxe, though Sasi pronounced as //qʼ// is sometimes slightly affricated. (The same pattern holds for Sasi contour clicks with pronounced as //qʼ//.) A contrastive pronounced as //qʼ// was reported by Gruber (1975) from a word or two in ǂHoan dialect, but could not be confirmed in Nǃaqriaxe, and cross-linguistic comparison gives reason to believe that pronounced as //qʼ// and pronounced as //qχʼ// are the same consonant.
pronounced as //dz// frequently appears as a fricative (pronounced as /[z]/ or further back). pronounced as //c// and pronounced as //ɟ// have a slightly fricated release, pronounced as /[cᶜ̧]/ or pronounced as /[kᶜ̧]/ etc., and aspirated pronounced as //cʰ// is distinguished primarily in the frication being longer than for pronounced as //c//. pronounced as //q// (pronounced as /[qᵡ]/) is similar, and pronounced as /[qχʼ]/ might be better analyzed as pronounced as //qʼ//. It is sometimes pronounced as a lateral pronounced as //q̠ʼ//, though not as commonly as in Gǀui. pronounced as //k// is not found in many lexical words apart from loans, but does occur in some highly frequent grammatical words. pronounced as //χ// is rare, and may be restricted to loans (this is not yet clear). Glottal stop pronounced as /[ʔ]/ could be argued to be epenthetic on an onsetless syllable rather than phonemic.
Consonants found in word-medial (C2) position are pronounced as //b// (often pronounced as /[β]/), pronounced as //m, n, ɾ//; the phonemic status of medial pronounced as /[w]/ in one word is unclear. pronounced as //ŋ// is rare, found word-finally in a few loanwords. An additional consonant, pronounced as //j//, is found as the first consonant of some grammatical markers.
The palatal series, which is most developed in ǂHȍã dialect, derive historically from dental consonants. This appears to be a regional influence from Gǀui, where it has also happened in some dialects more than other. Among ǂʼAmkoe dialects, there has been no palatalization in Sisa (pronounced as //n d t tʰ tʼ tχ tqχʼ//), palatalization of most alveolar consonants in Nǃaqriaxe (pronounced as //ɲ ɟ c cʰ cʼ tχ tqχʼ//), and full palatalization in ǂHoan (pronounced as //ɲ ɟ c cʰ cʼ cχ cqχʼ//). The shift of pronounced as //n/ > /ɲ// only took place in lexical words; in grammatical words, only pronounced as //n// is found.
/h/ is frequently a voiced (murmured) pronounced as /[ɦ]/, and has been described as being "absorbed" into the following vowel.
Like the Tuu languages, with which it was previously classified, ǂʼAmkoe has five click "types": bilabial, dental, alveolar, palatal, and lateral alveolar. There are 14 to 19 "accompaniments" (combinations of manner, phonation, and contour), depending on the speaker. As with non-clicks, the difference is in whether the speaker retains pre-voiced clicks like those found in the Ju languages and Taa. The result is 68 to 77 click consonants. (Theoretically, the numbers may be 70 and 95, as several clicks shown here were unattested in Gerlach 2012 but have since proven to be accidental gaps, and some or all of the gaps below are likely to be accidental as well. This is especially so considering that the pre-voiced clicks are attested from only a single speaker, for whom extensive data is not available, and that the delayed-aspirated series has not been reported from ǂHoan.) Gerlach (2015) finds the following inventory, considering Nǃaqriaxe and ǂHoan dialects:
'Noisy' clicks | 'Sharp' clicks | |||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
lateral | ||||||
Voiced nasal | pronounced as /link/ | pronounced as /link/ | pronounced as /link/ | pronounced as /link/ | pronounced as /link/ | |
Preglottalized voiced nasal | pronounced as /link/ | pronounced as /link/ | pronounced as /link/ | pronounced as /link/ | pronounced as /link/ | |
Voiced oral | pronounced as /link/ | pronounced as /link/ | pronounced as /link/ | pronounced as /link/ | pronounced as /link/ | |
Tenuis oral | pronounced as /link/ | pronounced as /link/ | pronounced as /link/ | pronounced as /link/ | pronounced as /link/ | |
Aspirated oral | pronounced as /link/ | pronounced as /link/ | pronounced as /link/ | pronounced as /link/ | pronounced as /link/ | |
Voiced aspirated oral | pronounced as /link/ | pronounced as /link/ | ||||
Ejective | pronounced as /link/ | pronounced as /link/ | pronounced as /link/ | pronounced as /link/ | pronounced as /link/ | |
Voiced ejective | pronounced as /link/ | pronounced as /link/ | pronounced as /link/ | |||
Glottalized (prenasalized between vowels) | pronounced as /link/ | pronounced as /link/ | pronounced as /link/ | pronounced as /link/ | pronounced as /link/ | |
Delayed aspiration (prenasalized between vowels) | pronounced as /link/ | pronounced as /link/ | pronounced as /link/ | |||
Contour clicks (uvular) | ||||||
Tenuis | pronounced as /ʘq/ | pronounced as /ǀq/ | pronounced as /ǁq/ | pronounced as /ǃq/ | pronounced as /ǂq/ | |
Voiced (sporadically prenasalized) | pronounced as /link/ | pronounced as /link/ | pronounced as /link/ | pronounced as /link/ | pronounced as /link/ | |
Aspirated | pronounced as /link/ | pronounced as /link/ | pronounced as /link/ | pronounced as /link/ | pronounced as /link/ | |
Voiced aspirated | pronounced as /link/ | |||||
Ejective | pronounced as /link/ | pronounced as /link/ | pronounced as /link/ | pronounced as /link/ | pronounced as /link/ | |
Voiced ejective | pronounced as /link/ | pronounced as /link/ | ||||
Affricate | (pronounced as /ʘχ/)? | (pronounced as /ǀχ/)? | (pronounced as /ǁχ/)? | (pronounced as /ǃχ/)? | (pronounced as /ǂχ/)? | |
Ejective affricate | pronounced as /link/ | pronounced as /link/ | pronounced as /link/ | pronounced as /link/ | pronounced as /link/ | |
Voiced ejective affricate | pronounced as /link/ | pronounced as /ᶢǁqχʼ/ | pronounced as /ᶢǂqχʼ/ |
The unusual distinction between glottalized and ejective clicks is similar to that found in Gǀui. A near minimal set is pronounced as /ǁʼòò/ 'warm', pronounced as /ᵑǁʔōō/ 'hard', pronounced as /ǁqʼòò/ 'to stink'. It has not been reported from ǂHoan, but this is likely to have been an oversight. The ejective clicks are not prenasalized between vowels, while the glottalized clicks, and clicks with delayed aspiration, are. Voicing of the voiced uvular clicks is variable. They are sporadically prenasalized, even in initial position, which many investigators believe is due to the difficulty of maintaining the voicing.
Like pronounced as //χ// etc. above, the plain click affricates pronounced as //ʘχ, ǀχ, ǁχ, ǃχ, ǂχ// appear to be found only in Gǀui loans.
With the voiceless aspirated clicks, voicing starts partway through the aspiration, pronounced as /[ǃʰʱ]/, so the voice-onset time is not as long as that of the glottalized clicks. With the voiced aspirated clicks, the aspiration is generally voiced throughout, but voicing decreases during the hold of the click, and the release itself is voiceless, unlike the release of modally voiced clicks. (That is, these are better described as pre-voiced aspirated clicks.) For the clicks with delayed aspiration, the aspiration is quite long, starting out weak and increasing in intensity with time (unlike the aspiration of the simple aspirated clicks, which starts out strong and decreases in intensity). When the click is in utterance-initial position, there is no voicing in the hold or in the aspiration. However, when the click occurs after a vowel, it is nasal throughout the hold, ending just before the release, but with voicing continuing though the release and throughout the aspiration: pronounced as /[ǃ˭ʰ]/ vs pronounced as /[ŋ͡nǃ̬ʱʱ]/. The preglottalized clicks have a much shorter voice lead (negative VOT) than the plain nasal clicks, sometimes hardly audible.
A lexical word is typically of the shapes CVV (69% in Nǃaqriaxe dialect), CVN (8%), or CVCV (22%, often loans from Gǀui), with two tone-bearing units. (Only 1% of words are CVCVCV, CVVCV, CVVVCV, and other complex patterns.) The N may only be pronounced as //m// in native words, though final pronounced as //n// occurs in loans. Gerlach (2015) believes that the CVV and CVN patterns derive historically from *CVCV through loss of C2 (such as a medial pronounced as //l// in related languages) or V2 in all cases, not just those that can be shown.
In lexical words, most consonants occur in C1 position, but only pronounced as //b m n r// occur in C2 position. pronounced as //ɾ// may be realized as pronounced as /[d]/ or pronounced as /[l]/, and pronounced as //b// may be realized as pronounced as /[β]/. (A few words have a third-position consonant, CVCVCV or CVVCV. These include pronounced as //b m r l k q ts s//, and may be a fossilized suffixes.) pronounced as //w//? occurs as C2 in one word, pronounced as //kawa// 'bag', but its analysis is uncertain - the word may be pronounced as //kaua//, with a CVVV structure, and perhaps a loan. pronounced as //m n ɾ// do not occur in C1 position except in loanwords. Initial pronounced as //ɾ// may be a trill pronounced as /[r]/ when it is a trill in the source language. pronounced as //b// occurs as C1 in only a few native words, pronounced as //ɲ// (C1 only) is rare, and final pronounced as //ŋ// is only found in loans. pronounced as //j// does not occur in lexical words, except for some speakers as the realization of pronounced as //ɲ//.
In grammatical words, the word shape is usually CV, sometimes CVV (generally shortening to CV in rapid speech), or, in two cases, N (both pronounced as //m// and pronounced as //n//). Attested consonants in grammatical words are pronounced as //ʔ j w m n k q s h ᵑǀ ᵑǃ ˀᵑǁ ǀʰ ǁ//. Of these, pronounced as //j w m n// do not occur as C1 in lexical words, while pronounced as //ʔ k// are rare. Thus there is a strong tendency for some consonants to mark the beginning of a lexical word, and for others to start grammatical words. (Though pronounced as //w// may be realized as pronounced as /[β]/ and might be phonemically pronounced as //b//.) In grammatical words, clicks are mostly found in CVV and CVq (pharyngealized) syllables, though there is a plural suffix pronounced as /-/ᵑǀe//. pronounced as //h// is sometimes pharyngealized to pronounced as /[ħ]/ in suffixes. At least pronounced as //χ, l, k, s, c// occur as C2 in loan words.
ǂHõã is an SVO subject–verb–object language (see examples in Collins 2001, 2002, 2003). The SVO word order of ǂHõã typical of the Kxʼa and Tuu language families. ǂHõã has nominal postpositions used for locative relations (see Collins 2001), and the possessor precedes the head noun.
ǂHõã grammar is characterized by a number of features common to the Kxʼa and Tuu languages. First there is an intricate system of nominal plurality and verbal pluractionality. Second, there is a system of verbal compounds. Third, there is a general-purpose preposition (referred to as the "linker" in Collins 2003) which appears between post-verbal constituents.